The Role of Religious Orientation and Ethical Ideologies in
Environmental Concerns amongst Teachers and School Staff in East
Java, Indonesia
Dexon Pasaribu
1a
, Pim Martens
2b
and Bagus Takwin
2c
1
Maastricht Sustainable Institute, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
2
Department of Social Psychology, Universitas Indonesia, Depok, Indonesia
Keywords: Religious Orientation, Ethical Ideology, Environmental Apathy, Environmental Concerns.
Abstract: Several studies show that more often than not, religion hinders the preservation awareness and efforts towards
the ecology. Others, however, have found that the belief in God or the identification with a particular religion
is not associated with measures for environmental concern. This study investigates how Allport’s intrinsic
personal (IP) and extrinsic social (ES) religious orientation and Forsyth’s ethical ideologies of idealism and
relativism relate to the measures of environmental concerns using ecocentric (EM), anthropocentric motives
(AM) and general environment apathy (GEA). Using quantitative design, we survey a total of 929 school
teachers and staff from 37 schools in East Java. Multiple regression is applied to analyse the data. Results
suggest mixed results whereby a higher IP more often leads to a lower GEA and a higher EM and AM. On
the other hand, relativism and ES consistently relate to a higher AM and a higher GEA. We also identify
different components of religious orientation which correlate significantly with idealism and relativism,
suggesting that individuals’ religious orientation may closely relates to their ethical belief and decision.
Lastly, several approaches to interpret the results along with several significant demographic and other
determinants with each of their limitations, are discussed.
1 INTRODUCTION
Religion has barely been featured amongst key
anthropogenic factors causing environmental
degradation (Bauman, Bohannon, & O’Brien, 2010);
at least not until after White's (1967) thesis about
religion gained sufficient attention from the scientific
community, where much of the later research would
then assume that religion and ecology are interrelated.
Several studies show that more often than not,
religion hinders the awareness of and efforts for
environmental sustainability, where it depresses
concern about the environment (Arbuckle & Konisky,
2015; Barker & Bearce, 2013; Muñoz-García, 2014).
Others, however, have found that the belief in God or
the identification with a particular religion is not
associated with measures of environmental concern
(Boyd, 1999; Hayes & Marangudakis, 2000, 2001;
Smith & Leiserowitz, 2013).
There are several possible reasons for these mixed
results. One reason might stem from how each study
addresses different aspects and properties of religion
in measuring religious value, such as religious
scriptures, contents and interpretation (Haq, 2001;
McFague, 2001; Tirosh-Samuelson, 2001), or
communication framing (Smith & Leiserowitz, 2013;
Wardekker, Petersen, & van der Sluijs, 2009).
Another reason might reside in how various studies
differ in how they define religiosity, religiousness or
religious belief. Gallagher & Tierney (Gallagher &
Tierney, 2013) argue that religiosity and religiousness
are interchangeable as far an individual’s conviction,
devotion and veneration towards a divinity is
concerned. However, religiosity or religiousness can
be broadly or narrowly formulated using differing
aspects such as (1) human cognitive aspect (beliefs,
knowledge), (2) affect, which relates emotions to
religion, and (3) behavior, such as time spent praying
or reading religious texts, attendance, or affiliation
(Cornwall, 1989). Thus, differing foci and aspects
produced various operationalizations of religiosity,
such as religious orthodoxy (Fullerton & Hunsberger,
1982; Hunsberger, 1989), typology (Glock & Stark,
1965), fundamentalism (Kellstedt & Smidt, 1991;
Pasaribu, D., Martens, P. and Takwin, B.
The Role of Religious Orientation and Ethical Ideologies in Environmental Concerns amongst Teachers and School Staff in East Java, Indonesia.
DOI: 10.5220/0010755400003112
In Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences (ICE-HUMS 2021), pages 551-573
ISBN: 978-989-758-604-0
Copyright
c
2022 by SCITEPRESS Science and Technology Publications, Lda. All rights reserved
551
McFarland, 1989), and religious orientation (Allport,
1966; Allport & Ross, 1967; Donahue, 1985). For
religious belief, this study views Allport’s religious
orientation fits well in defining the interchangeably-
used religiosity or religiousness, as far as it
approaches beliefs, knowledge and affectation of
intrinsic, extrinsic personal and extrinsic social
motivation in engaging in religious activities. In
detail, Allport’s religious orientation consists of
intrinsic religious orientation, where religion is
deeply personal to the individual, such as the
commitment to a religious life and living out his/her
religion; extrinsic personal religious orientation, with
religion being a source of peace safety and comfort,
which is a direct result of participating in religious
activity; and, finally, extrinsic social religious
orientation, where the emphasis is placed on religion
as membership in a powerful in-group, providing
protection, consolation or social status, and enabling
religious participation (Allport & Ross, 1967; Fleck,
1981; Genia & Shaw, 1991; Kahoe & Meadow, 1981;
Maltby, 1999).
The present study proposes to address religion as
a major driver of ethics and how it relates to attitudes
towards the natural environment preservation and
sustainability. Studies examining the relationship
between religious belief and ethical ideologies
(Cornwell et al., 2005; Watson, Morris, Hood,
Milliron, & Stutz, 1998; Weaver & Agle, 2002)
provide evidence that ethical ideologies facilitate
broader philosophical coverage corresponding to
religious values and beliefs. Several studies argue that
general spiritual principles and values are largely
related to ethics (Cornwell et al., 2005; Jackson,
1999; Skipper & Hyman, 1993), indicating that
religiosity significantly correlates with Forsyth
(1980) idealist and anti-relativist ethical ideologies
(Barnett, Bass, & Brown, 1996; Watson et al., 1998).
Cornwell et al. (1994) found that religion has some
effect on ethical positions. Austrian Christians are
significantly less idealistic and relativistic than all
other religions, even with other Christians from the
United States and Britain. They argued that there are
some ethical convergence between religions. In
another study, Barnett et al. (1996) concluded that
religiosity correlates positively with a non-relativist
ethical ideology. Closely similar with them, Watson
et al. (1998) argued that religious intrinsicness or
religious intrinsic personal orientation is associated
with the idealism and antirelativism of an absolutist
ethical position. They argued that intrinsic
commitments to religion may simply mean that
certain beliefs are absolutely non-negotiable (Watson
et al., 1998, p. 5). In Forsyth's (1980) terms, this
absolutistic way of thinking type is the result when
people strongly believe that moral decision should be
guided by an universal governing principle (low
relativism) rather than by personal or situational
analysis (high relativism) while also convinced that
ethical behavior will always lead to positive
consequences.
Forsyth (1980) ethical ideologies consist of two
components, namely, ethical idealism and ethical
relativism. An idealist thinks that ethical behavior
will always lead to positive consequences, while a
relativist rejects universal moral principles, instead
believing that moral decisions should be based on a
personal or situational analysis (Forsyth, 1980).
Nonetheless, the role religion plays to the concerns
for ecology is as yet still unclear. Studies on ethical
ideologies provide clear evidence where religiosity
significantly correlates with idealism and anti-
relativism (Barnett et al., 1996; Watson et al., 1998).
Thus, combining results from above mentioned
studies, the present study targets religious orientation
and ethical ideologies as the main variables to explore
how both religiousness and ethic relate and interact
with concerns for the natural environment
preservation. For the first working hypothesis, this
study predicts that intrinsic personal religious
orientations has a positive correlation with ethical
idealism and a negative correlation with relativism.
For sustainability and the attitude or concerns to
the natural environment, White (1967) arguments
highlight the urge for sustainability in responding
development and growth at that time. White (1967)
argues that, to some extent, the current ecological
crisis is due to the disconnection of nature and
spirituality often promoted by religion which gives
the human species rights and dominance to exploit
nature which forms the basis for exploiting the natural
world. The concept of Sustainable Development first
became prominent in the 1980s with its most
mainstream definition of “development that meets the
needs of the present without compromising the ability
of future generations to meet their own needs”
(Brundtland, 1987). From this definition, three pillars
approach derived consisting social sustainability,
economic sustainability, and environmental
sustainability. In its progression, the latter mainly
become the domain of sustainability sciences while
the former two (namely, economic and social
sustainability) have mainly become the domain of
development studies.
In contrast, despite efforts to incorporate research
results from both development and sustainability
disciplines, a complete integration to achieve
sustainable development is facing numerous
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
552
challenges. According to Goodland & Daly (1996),
one of the problems is because of the difference in
priorities in both disciplines. While the development
goals are fundamentally important, they are quite
different from the goals of environmental
sustainability, which is the unimpaired maintenance
of human life-support systems Goodland (1995, p. 5).
Goodland & Daly (1996) differentiate, at the very
least, four kinds of capital which are human-made
capital (the one usually considered in financial and
economic accounts); natural capital (the stock of
environmentally provided assets such as soil,
atmosphere, forests, water, wetlands); human capital
(investments in education, health and nutrition of
individuals); and social capital (the institutional and
cultural basis for a society to function). Goodland &
Daly (1996) challenge the notion of throughput
growth in the context of finite earth, in which as a
subsystem of the finite and non-growing earth, the
economy must eventually adapt to it. To emphasize
this finite earth, they further challenge the economic
concept of ‘income’ arguing that “any consumption
that is based on the depletion of natural capital should
not be counted as income.” Prevailing models of
economic analysis tend to treat consumption of
natural capital as income and therefore tend to
promote patterns of economic activity that are
unsustainable. Consumption of natural capital is a
liquidation, the opposite of capital accumulation”
(Goodland & Daly, 1996, p. 1005). Thus,
environmental sustainability requires maintaining
natural capital; and to understand it includes defining
"natural capital" and "maintenance of resources" (or
at least "non-declining levels of resources").
Sustainability means maintaining environmental
assets, or at least not depleting them. Goodland &
Daly (1996) argue that the limiting factor for much
economic development has become natural capital as
much as human-made capital.In some cases, like
marine fishing, it has become the limiting factor—
fish have become limiting, rather than fishing boats.
Timber is limited by remaining forests, not by
sawmills; petroleum is limited by geological deposits
and atmospheric capacity to absorb CO2, not by
refining capacity” (Goodland & Daly, 1996, p. 1005).
In this sense of finite natural capital, they also
introduced cultivated natural capital (such as
agriculture products, pond-bred fish, cattle herds, and
plantation forests)—the combination of natural and
human-made capital— which dramatically expands
the capacity of natural capital to deliver services.
Nevertheless, Goodland & Daly (1996) concludes
that eventually, natural capital will limit this
cultivated natural capital.
In support to Goodland (1995) and Goodland &
Daly (1996), the present study bring forth the
dilemma between sustainability science and
development studies whereby they haven’t yet
reached consensus on the attainable priorities path-
ways on whether to reach environmental
sustainability or more anthropocentric (social and
economic) sustainability. Similarly, Thompson &
Barton (1994) formulated and developed two
underlying motives of environmental attitudes, which
are ecocentrism—valuing nature for its own sake; and
anthropocentrism—valuing nature because of the
material or physical benefits it provides; with an
additional dimension of general apathy towards the
environment (Gardner & Stern, Stern & Dietz,
Oksanen, as cited in Bjerke & Kaltenborn, 1999).
Thompson & Barton (1994) proposed that the
motives and values which underlie environmental
attitudes are of great significance in which the same
positive attitude to the importance and conservation
of the natural environment might come from
ecocentric or anthropocentric motives, or even both,
making the importance of general environment
apathy scale as one strong potential cross-section
predictor for both environmental attitude and
acceptability of harming animal. This is especially
relevant after Bjerke and Kaltenborn (1999) further
riddled this topic when they found that ecocentric
motives scored differently to different job-groups
categorization when valuing carnivores animals
compared to herbivores. In their study of ecocentric
and anthropocentric motives relationship to attitudes
towards large carnivores, Bjerke and Kaltenborn
(1999) highlighted that high ecocentrism and low
apathy to the natural environment only specifically
resonate to those research biologist and wildlife
managers groups who scored positive attitude
towards carnivores. Thus as the second working
hypothesis, the present study proposes Thompson &
Barton's (1994) general environmental apathy scale
will negatively correlated with ecocentric and
anthropocentric motives.
While there are ample studies connecting religion
either to ethical ideologies or to environmental
sustainability, studies examining both ethical
ideologies and environment sustainability at once, are
lacking. One exception is in the field of animal
welfare, where there are a growing number of
investigations confirming positive correlation
between ethical ideologies and public’s attitudes
towards animals (Galvin & Herzog, 1992; Herzog &
Nickell, 1996; B Su & Martens, 2017; Bingtao Su &
Martens, 2018; Wuensch, Jenkins, & Poteat, 2002).
Studies of ethical ideologies and attitudes towards
The Role of Religious Orientation and Ethical Ideologies in Environmental Concerns amongst Teachers and School Staff in East Java,
Indonesia
553
animals and animal protection demonstrate that
public’s attitudes towards animals or animal
experiments are related to their ethical perspectives.
One study investigating the role of idealism and
relativism in research using animal in the United
States demonstrates that idealism correlates
negatively and relativism correlates positively to
support for animal research (Wuensch & Poteat,
1998). They argued that idealists often express
greater moral concern for how animals are utilized
than their relativist counterparts (Wuensch & Poteat,
1998). Specifically for Forsyth’s idealism, later
studies provide more evidence that positive attitudes
to animals correlate positively to ethical idealism,
where people's moral idealism significantly
influences their attitudes towards animals (Galvin &
Herzog, 1992; B Su & Martens, 2017). Galvin &
Herzog (1992) found that ethical idealism relates
positively to a higher concern for animal use.
Through their research about the effectiveness of
materials designed to sway public’s opinion about
biomedical research using animals, Herzog & Nickell
(1996) would later add that compared to males and
those low in ethical idealism, females and subjects
high in moral idealism rate higher effectiveness to
those research materials and advertising that reject
animal use in biomedical research (anti-animal
research materials) (p. 9). More recent studies by B
Su & Martens (2017, 2018) also confirmed these
results, showing that higher idealism scorers are more
likely to have a more positive attitude to animals and
a lower acceptability for harming animals. The more
those individuals consider their ethical behavior
would always lead to desirable consequences, the
more they appreciate animals (B Su & Martens,
2017). At the very least, it has been consistently
proven that ethical idealism lowers acceptability for
harming animals, instead encouraging more positive
attitudes towards animal (Galvin & Herzog, 1992; B
Su & Martens, 2017; Bingtao Su & Martens, 2018).
There was not much support for the significance of
relativism except only from Wuensch & Poteat
(1998) who found that higher score of relativism
relates to higher support for research using animals.
However B Su & Martens (2017, 2018) slightly
deviate from older studies (Galvin & Herzog, 1992;
Herzog & Nickell, 1996) whereby they find that high
scorers of ethical relativism are more likely to have a
more negative attitude towards animals only in China
(B Su & Martens, 2017), but not in their Dutch
sample (Bingtao Su & Martens, 2018). B Su &
Martens (2017, 2018) argued that the differences
between both samples may stem from the difference
between being a developed and developing country,
respectively. However, despite this slight difference,
most animal welfare studies examining the role of
ethical ideologies showed that ethical idealism and
relativism relates to people’s attitude towards and
acceptability for harming animal. Thus, incorporating
previous research results from the field of animal
welfare, this study tries to carefully simulate for
whether those findings from animal welfare studies
also replicate to the attitude to the natural
environment preservation.
Bjerke and Kaltenborn (1999) argued that positive
attitudes towards animals may stem from either
anthropocentric or ecocentric motives or both. The
present study considers these ecocentric and
anthropocentric value and motives to be particularly
important partly as the results of ethical idealism and
ethical relativism ideologies. Borrowing findings
from previously mentioned animal welfare studies
(Galvin & Herzog, 1992; B Su & Martens, 2017;
Bingtao Su & Martens, 2018; Wuensch & Poteat,
1998), this study tries to extend those results into a
more general environmental preservation concerns. A
person highly views that his/her ethical behavior will
always lead to positive consequences and who also
firmly believes that there are universal moral
principles (low relativism), may weighs more to
higher environmental concerns in perceiving his/her
surroundings. On the other hand, a person who views
that his/her ethical behavior will not always lead to
positive consequences (low idealism) while also
firmly believes that there are no governing universal
moral principles (high relativism) may weigh in more
to a lower environmental concerns. Therefore, the
third working hypothesis of this study predicts that
higher environmental concern correlates positively
with ethical idealism and negatively with relativism.
In more detail, this study proposes that individual
with higher environmental concerns are those
participants who scored a lower general
environmental apathy and a higher ecocentric
motives in valuing the natural environment. And
such, taking together as well as independently, lower
general environment apathy and higher ecocentric
motives should relate to a higher idealism and a lower
relativism. Thus, for the third hypothesis the opposite
should also true, whereby a higher general
environmental apathy and a lower ecocentric motives
in valuing the natural environment should relate with
a lower idealism and a higher relativism.
In addition, using the context of White's (1967)
perspectives, the present study aims to further
examine the relation between religion (i.e. both as
cognitive belief and ethical judgment) and the attitude
to the importance and conservation of the natural
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
554
environment. Allport & Ross (1967) religious
orientation construct has been chosen to measure
religious intrinsic, extrinsic personal and extrinsic
social orientations. In later developments of religious
orientation, the dimension of extrinsic social motives
has been added (Donahue, 1985; Maltby, 1999;
Trimble, 1997). Extrinsic social religious orientation
addresses how individuals practice religion more as
an instrument for social gain such as membership in
a powerful in-group, providing protection,
consolation or social status, and enabling religious
participation. The extrinsic social religious
orientation is more closely related to the social
identity in-group membership concept (Henri Tajfel,
1974, 1981; Turner, 1975) which introduce
instrumental views of religion for social gain whereby
religious belief systems are used to obtain desirable
outcomes that may unnecessarily be ethical or
unethical. On one hand, the ethical means for social
gain may very much corresponds to the concept of
ethical idealism where ethical behaviour is believed
will always bring positive outcome. However, on the
other hand, should there be unethical means for social
gains, it may relate to lower idealism, and higher
relativism in which a person strongly believe that
there is no universal moral standard, and therefore,
moral decisions should be based on the personal or
situational analysis. In this sense, we are carefully
posing the working hypothesis for the relationship
between extrinsic social religious orientation and
ethical ideologies. Therefore, as the fourth
hypothesis, we predict that higher extrinsic social
religious orientation relates to a lower idealism and
higher relativism. This hypothesis is an extension
from the first hypothesis, in which we seek to find
evidence of how religious orientation relates to the
natural environment preservation attitude by
examining how it correlates to ethical ideologies.
Lastly, as previously in the third hypothesis we
predict that higher relativism relates to a higher
environmental apathy, for the fifth hypothesis, this
study expects that a higher extrinsic social religious
orientation will also relate to a higher environmental
apathy.
It is important to emphasize that this study is not
theological in nature and is not describing Islamic
religious worldview of the natural environment. As
previously discussed, this study approaches the
religious belief through Allport & Ross' (1967)
religious orientation. Specifically for extrinsic social
religious orientation (ES), we argue that it strongly
overlaps with the social identity in-group
membership theory (Henri Tajfel, 1974, 1981;
Turner, 1975) especially in the concept of social
category. In this study, we view that the extrinsic
social religious orientation echoes a social category
notion that offers a sense of identity which
individuals identify with and act in the ways they
believe represent their group’s identity (Blumer,
1958; Henry Tajfel & Turner, 1979). Individuals who
identify themselves as Muslims are more likely to
behave in accordance with the typical behaviours of
fellow Muslims. Therefore, this study purposefully
selects the population in East Java province,
considering that it represents some of the oldest, most
influential Islamic communities and organizations,
whilst also being the province with the most diverse
Islamic denomination.
The province of East Java is the birthplace of
Nahdlatul Ulama (NU), the largest Islamic mass
organization in Indonesia. It has approximately 40
million members throughout the nation and its
influence is not merely at the regency-level but also
at the national (Anwar, 2019). Secondly, East Java is
well-known for its long history of Islamic boarding
schools. Pesantren Darul Ulum is one of the oldest
and most distinguished in Jombang, East Java
(Turmudi, 2006). Thirdly, East Java offers an
interesting segment of the political constellation in
Indonesia. Its political influence at the national level
has been prominent since the making of the nation
(Bush, 2009). Two of the most renowned instances
were the appointment of Abdurrahman Wahid as the
fourth President of Indonesia (1999-2001) and the
appointment of Ma’ruf Amin as the current
Indonesian vice president (took office in 2019), both
of whom have strong ties to Nahdlatul Ulama in East
Java. All in all, the above reasons foster East Java as
one of the most relevant candidate-grounds for
scrutinizing the relationship between religiousness
and the attitudes held towards the importance of
natural environment preservation; moreover, due to
the religious groups’ prevalence in East Java, we
should point out that our respondents are likely to be
Muslims. Regardless of all the above, however close
a representation East Java is of the everyday major
religious worldview in Indonesia, the present study
avoids over-generalization of the results representing
the whole country.
This study targeted school teacher and staff in
viewing that as an institution, both public and private
schools are subjects to nation-wide education
curriculum whereby collected data may generally
capture a nation-wide curriculum’s learning goals
(Swirski, 2002) relevant to natural environment
protection. However, there were also a lengthy
discussions about educators roles as transformative
intellectuals rather than as nation-state agent teaching
The Role of Religious Orientation and Ethical Ideologies in Environmental Concerns amongst Teachers and School Staff in East Java,
Indonesia
555
nation-state learning goals (Leite, Fernandes, &
Figueiredo, 2020; Muff & Bekerman, 2019; Tan,
2016). Also, taking some roles and responsibilities of
a parent (loco parentis), teacher may be as well
provide assistance and insight on moral, political,
religious and ethical issues for their students (Grubb,
1995) as one study hinted that teachers act as role-
models for the students and influence their students’
political attitudes (Bar-Tal & Harel, 2002).
In other study related to transformative agency,
teachers’ inclusive practices, moral purposes,
competence, autonomy and reflexivity (Pantić, 2015)
are important factors to act as an agent of change. The
duality of being transformational agents while also
fulfilling their obligatory role to implement the
nation-state education curriculum agenda, Muff &
Bekerman (2019) argued that teachers mediate their
roles between the different demands that of the civic
education politics impose to them by navigating
elegantly both in producing hegemonic discourse and
in fostering ways to rebel against and draw counter-
hegemonic strategies in their classroom practice.
Thus, this study viewed that having teachers as the
participants for the research would capture some
dynamics of interlocking roles at play. To name a
few, the nation-state curriculum goals, teachers’
beliefs, moral purposes, reflexivity and awareness in
responding to the nation-state curriculum, and their
combined roles as transformative intellectuals, more
or less, are the dynamics reflected in classroom
discourses. Teachers attitudes to the preservation and
protection to the natural environment may best
represent the nation’s sets of environmental policy
and the younger generation’s perspective.
Lastly, we also emphasize the demographic
determinants commonly suggested in most studies
about religion and ethical ideologies, such as gender,
age, household income, education, pet ownership,
religious organization affiliation, meat consumption
(B Su & Martens, 2017; Bingtao Su & Martens,
2018). We will therefore closely scrutinize these
important demographic or other determinants in our
analysis.
2 MATERIAL AND METHODS
This research targeted Muslim teachers and school
staff in the province of East Java, Indonesia, using
cluster sampling, whereby a paper and pencil survey
of teachers was conducted. Survey participation
invitations were sent to 67 schools (ranging from
junior to senior high schools). The survey invitation
emphasizes that it is important for the school to
provide a balanced proportion of male and female
teachers or school staff. Total of 37 schools, from 10
districts of East Java, replied and agreed to
participate, providing 1007 participants. However,
only 929 participants were analysed due to removing
78 participants because of incomplete and unengaged
answers (see section 3.2).
All the questionnaires in the survey were
originally in English. We then translated them to
Indonesian. The method of translation and adaptation
was using expert judgement and back translation. The
questionnaires were translated to Bahasa Indonesia
and sent to experts for evaluation and finalization of
the translation. After corrections, the questionnaires
were translated back to English by three Indonesian
academicians from Universitas Indonesia. Back-
translated items that are very similar to their English
language origin are retained, and the remaining are
modified or deleted.
The set of questionnaires consist of four sections.
In the first section, we asked a variety of important
determinants and demographic details such as birth
year (age), gender, highest level of education
completed, their household composition (for
example, single, married, or widow(er), with children
or not), place of residence (rural or urban), type of
house (apartment, live with parents, etc.), their
opinion regarding the importance of
religion/spirituality in their lives, their experience or
participation in religious organization, household
income, pet ownership, kinds of pet, their weekly
frequency of meat consumption, and the frequency of
visiting public zoos or aquariums in a year.
In the second section, Thompson & Barton's
(1994) Ecocentric-Anthropocentric Scale of
Environmental Attitude (EASEA) is used to measure
environmental motives and apathy. There are 30-
items rated on a five-point scale ranging from one,
extremely disagree, to five, extremely agree. In order
to translate and adapt this questionnaire into
Indonesia language, we feel necessary to translate a
question into two forms, which in turn make the
resulting Indonesian version to total 31-items. A high
score on a question indicates a high level of
agreeableness for the topic, which basically consist of
three dimensions. The first measures ecocentric
motive where nature is valued for its own sake, and
therefore, judged that it deserves protection because
of its intrinsic value. The type of issue statement
being asked are, for example, ‘I can enjoy spending
time in natural settings just for the sake of being out
in nature,’ Sometimes animals seem almost human
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
556
Table 1: EASEA-ecocentric rotated factor matrix.
Items
Facto
r
a
1 2
ECCANTH02 I en
j
o
y
s
p
endin
g
time in natural settin
g
s
j
ust for the sake of bein
g
out in nature .464
ECCANTH12 I need time in nature to be happ
y
.608
ECCANTH15 Sometimes when I am unhappy I find comfort in nature .622
ECCANTH26 Being out in nature is a great stress reducer for me .666
ECCANTH28 One of the most im
p
ortant reasons to conserve is to
p
reserve wild areas .428
ECCANTH30 Sometimes animals seem almost human to me .616
ECCANTH31 Human are as much a
p
art of the ecos
y
stem as other animals .612
Extraction Method: Principal Axis Factoring. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.
Table 2: EASEA-anthropocentric rotated factor matrix.
Items
Model 1 (using
ei
g
en value > 1
)
Model 2 (as one
factor)
1 2
ECCANTH04 The worst thing about the loss of the rain forest is that it will
restrict the develo
p
ment of new medicines
.771 Delete
ECCANTH05 The worst thing about the loss of the rain forest is that it will
reduce plants and animals which benefit for human kin
.497 .414
ECCANTH20 The most important reason for conservation is human
survival
.510
.429
ECCANTH22 Nature is important because of what it can contribute to the
p
leasure and welfare of humans
.611
.564
ECCANTH25 We need to preserve resources to maintain a high quality of
life
.600 .567
ECCANTH27 One of the most important reasons to conserve is to ensure a
continued hi
g
h standard of livin
g
.429 .563
ECCANTH29 Continued land development is a good idea as long as a high
quality of life can be preserve
d
.412 .501
Extraction Method: Principal Axis Factoring. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.
to me,’ or ‘Nature is valuable for its own sake.’ There
are total of 12 questions in the ecocentric dimension.
However, after principal axis factoring factor
analysis, this study not only reduced the items to only
seven items, but also found that the ecocentric
dimension consists of two factors (Table 1). The two-
factors findings of this study may confirm Amérigo et
al., (2007) which argue that ecocentrism seems to
include two concepts: the self in nature
(egobiocentrism) and nature itself (biospherism). In
ecocentrism motives, on the one hand, there are items
about physical or psychological benefits for the
individual, brought about by the mere fact of being in
or thinking about nature (e.g. “Being out in nature is
a great stress reducer for me). These are related to
the positive emotional effects produced by contact
with nature where the protagonist is the self and it is
the only direct beneficiary of the goodness of the
natural environment which could be considered to be
related to an egoistic dimension (Amérigo et al.,
2007). On the other hand, the remaining ecocentric
items refer to biospheric aspects that emphasize the
intrinsic value of Nature (e.g. “Nature is valuable for
its own sake”) which may be oriented to two different
viewpoints of (a) a psychosocial perspective that
contemplates the human-being-in-nature and in
which the environment is valued as an element that
procures the individual’s physical and psychological
well- being, and (b) a strictly biospheric dimension in
which the environment is valued intrinsically and that
contemplates the nonhuman elements of nature
(Amérigo et al., 2007). The present study addresses
item 2, 12, 15, and 26 as those from the
egobiocentrism factor while the remaining are those
closely related to biospherism factor.
The second measures anthropocentric motive
where the natural environment is valued due to its
importance in maintaining or enhancing the quality of
life for humankind and therefore should be protected
(Thompson & Barton, 1994, p.149). The type of issue
statement being asked are, for example, ‘the most
important reason for conservation is human survival,’
The Role of Religious Orientation and Ethical Ideologies in Environmental Concerns amongst Teachers and School Staff in East Java,
Indonesia
557
Table 3: EASEA-general environment apathy rotated factor matrix.
Items
Model 1 (using
eigen value > 1)
Model 2 (as one
factor)
1 2
ECCANTH03 Environmental threats such as deforestation and ozone
depletion have been exaggerate
d
.462
.518
ECCANTH07 It seems to me that most conservationists are pessimistic and
somewhat
p
aranoid.
.535
.594
ECCANTH09 I do not think the problem of depletion of natural resources
is as as bad as man
y
p
eo
p
le make it out to be
.692
.651
ECCANTH10 I find it hard to get too concerned about environmental
issues
.721
.611
ECCANTH14 I do not feel that humans are dependent on nature to survive .445 .545
ECCANTH17 I don't care about environmental problems .746 .549
ECCANTH18 I'm opposed to programs to preserve wilderness, reduce
p
ollution and conserve resources
.683 .591
Extraction Method: Principal Axis Factoring. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.
‘we need to preserve resources to maintain a high
quality of life,’ or ‘one of the best things about
recycling is that it saves money.’ There are total of 10
questions in the anthropocentric motive dimension.
However, after principal axis factoring factor
analysis, this study not only reduced the items to only
seven items, but also found that the anthropocentric
motives dimension consisted of two factors (Table 2).
The outcome of two-factors anthropocentric motives
are unexpected considering that item 5 was not an
original item rather than a new one created in order to
give a clear, simple to understand Indonesia
translation of item 4. We assumed that the second
factor (consisted of only item 4 and 5) might emerge
because of the similarity of the statement and the
order of appearance next to each other in the
questionnaire. This may give involuntary needs for
consistency to the participants when answering item
5 after they finish answering the previous one (item
4). After reliability analysis, this study decided to use
Model 2 anthropocentric scale using only 6 items
(5,20,22,25,27,29).
Lastly, the third-dimension measures general
apathy to the natural environment. The type of issue
statement being asked are, for example,
‘environmental threats such as deforestation and
ozone depletion have been exaggerated,’ too much
emphasis has been placed on conservation,’ or ‘I don't
care about environmental problems.’ There are total
of nine questions in the anthropocentric motive
dimension. However, after principal axis factoring
factor analysis, this study not only reduced the items
to only seven items, but also found that the apathy
dimension consisted of two factors instead of one.
However, after ensuring a relatively stable Cronbach
alpha’s reliability in one factor model, the present
study decided to retain the environmental apathy
dimension as it was originally, a one factor construct
(model two, see Table 3).
In the third section, the Religious Orientation
Scale (ROS) (Allport, 1966; Allport & Ross, 1967;
Leong & Zachar, 1990) was originally used to
measure intrinsic and extrinsic religious orientation.
We used Maltby's (1999) 15-item version which
incorporated Kirkpatrick's (1999) analysis expanding
ROS into three scales: intrinsic orientation (IP),
extrinsic personal—religion as a source of comfort
(EP) and extrinsic social—religion as social gain
(ES). The 15-item scale therefore consists of nine
questions addressing IP, for example, ’I try hard to
live all my life according to my religious beliefs’,
’My whole approach to life is based on my religion’,
’It is important to me to spend time in private thought
and prayer’); three questions addressing EP, for
example ‘Prayer is for peace and happiness’, ‘I pray
mainly to gain relief and protection’; and lastly, the
remaining three covering the ES dimension, for
example, ‘I go to church because it helps me make
friends’, ‘I go to church mainly because I enjoy
seeing people I know there’. However, after principal
axis factoring factor analysis, the present study found
only two dimensions of intrinsic personal (IP) and
extrinsic social (ES). After factor analysis, the EP was
accounted as the same factor as IP (Table 4), and thus,
will be considered as the same as IP.
In the fourth section, the Ethical Position
Questionnaire (EPQ) was used to measure the
differences in personal moral philosophy (Forsyth,
1980; Galvin & Herzog, 1992). The original EPQ was
a 20-items Likert scale consist of two sub-scales.
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
558
Table 4: ROS rotated factor matrix
Items
Facto
r
1 2
ROS01
(
IP
)
I tr
y
hard to live all m
y
life accordin
g
to m
y
reli
g
ious beliefs .673
ROS03 (IP) I have often had a strong sense of God’s presence .608
ROS04 (IP) My whole approach to life is based on my religion .705
ROS05 (IP) Prayers I say when I’m alone are as important as those I say in church .577
ROS06
(
IP
)
I attend church once a week or more .358
ROS07
(
IP
)
M
y
reli
g
ion is im
p
ortant because it answers man
y
q
uestions about the meanin
g
of life .741
ROS08
(
IP
)
I en
j
o
y
readin
g
about m
y
reli
g
ion .750
ROS09 (IP) It is important to me to spend time in private thought and
p
raye
r
.630
ROS10 (EP) What religion offers me most is comfort in times of trouble and sorrow .665
ROS11 (EP) Prayer is for peace and happiness .764
ROS12 (EP) I pray mainly to gain relief and protection .622
ROS13
(
ES
)
I
g
o to church
b
ecause it hel
p
s me make friends .833
ROS14
(
ES
)
I
g
o to church mainl
y
because I en
j
o
y
seein
g
p
eo
p
le I know there .894
ROS15
(
ES
)
I
g
o to church mostl
y
to s
p
end time with m
y
friends .787
Extraction Method: Principal Axis Factoring. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.
The first 10 items were designed to measure the
ethical idealism dimension, while the last 10 items
measured ethical relativism. Respondents were asked
to respond to statement using the nine-point EPQ
ranging from one (completely disagree) to nine
(completely agree). Regarding the ethical idealism,
six items were removed from analysis of this study.
Four out of those six items were removed because of
significant skew values which were outside the range
between -2 to 2 (Kim, 2013). The remaining two were
removed because of low factor loading, along with
three items from ethical relativism. After principal
axis factoring factor analysis, the present study uses
only 11 EPQ items. In which four items from the
idealism scale, and seven items from the relativism
scale. Factor analysis also found that the remaining
seven items of ethical relativism were put into two
factors. However, after ensuring a relatively stable
Cronbach alpha’s reliability in one factor model, the
present study decided to retain ethical relativism as it
was, a one factor construct (model two, see Table 5).
Statistical analysis
Religious orientation, ethical ideologies and
EASEA were analysed with IBM SPSS 24 using
multiple regression statistical procedures. This study
also used Pearson correlation product moment in
investigating the relation between religious
orientation and ethical ideologies. The resulting
correlation tables provides additional explanation for
the multiple regression results.
One common method examining EPQ were
conducted using ANOVA design (B Su & Martens,
2017; Bingtao Su & Martens, 2018), where EPQ was
considered as categorical variables differentiated into
four groups depending on the high and low of each
ethical idealism and relativism score. These groups
are, situationists (high idealism and high relativism),
subjectivists (low idealism and high relativism),
absolutists (high idealism and low relativism) and
exceptionists (low idealism and low relativism)
(Figure 1). In this study however, we view that it is
best to retain the interval properties from the total
score of ethical idealism and relativism to provide
richer and a more detailed data. Thus, multiple
regression is our selected statistical procedure for the
given data.
Figure 1: Ethical positions according idealism and
relativism.
This study uses two models of multiple
regression. The first model only investigates the main
variables, while the second model takes all main
variables with the demographic and other important
determinants. For both of the regression models, this
The Role of Religious Orientation and Ethical Ideologies in Environmental Concerns amongst Teachers and School Staff in East Java,
Indonesia
559
Table 5: EPQ pattern matrix.
Model 1 (using eigen
value > 1)
Model 2 (forced
as 2 factor
loadin
g
s
)
1 2 3 1 2
EPQ02 (I) Risks to another should never be tolerated, irrespective of
how small the risks might be.
.551
.549
EPQ03 (I) The existence of potential harm to others is always wrong,
irrespective of the benefits to be gained.
.651
.656
EPQ08 (I) The dignity and welfare of the people should be the most
important concern in any society.
.581
.580
EPQ10 (I) Moral behaviors are actions that closely match ideals of
the most “perfect” action.
.465
.463
EPQ15 (R) Questions of what is ethical for everyone can never be
resolved since what is moral or immoral is up to the individual.
.650
.603
EPQ16 (R) Moral standards are simply personal rules that indicate
how a person should behave and are not be be applied in making
judgments of others.
.704
.589
EPQ17 (R) Ethical considerations in interpersonal relations are so
complex that individuals should be allowed to formulate their own
individual codes.
.712
.742
EPQ18 (R) Rigidly codifying an ethical position that prevents certain
types of actions could stand in the way of better human relations and
adjustment.
.425
.561
EPQ19 (R) No rule concerning lying can be formulated; whether a lie
is permissible or not permissible totally depends upon the situation.
.762
.673
EPQ20 (R) Whether a lie is judged to be moral or immoral depends
upon the circumstances surrounding the action.
.748
.600
Extraction Method: Principal Axis Factoring. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.
Table 6: Skewness and kurtosis value of main variables.
N Skewness Kurtosis
Statistic Statistic Std. Erro
r
Statistic Std. Erro
r
EASEA-Ecocentric E
g
obios
p
her
(
EEM
)
929 -.422 .080 .556 .160
EASEA-Ecocentric Biosphere (EBM) 929 -.469 .080 .876 .160
EASEA-Anthro
p
ocentric Motives
(
AM
)
929 -.505 .080 1.298 .160
EASEA-General environment Apathy (GEA) 929 .343 .080 -.119 .160
EPQ Idealis
m
929 -1.196 .080 1.162 .160
EPQ Relativis
m
929 -.568 .080 -.017 .160
ROS Intrinsic Personal 929 -.751 .080 1.430 .160
ROS_Extrinsic Social 929 .195 .080 -.495 .160
Valid N
(
listwise
)
929
study avoids stepwise method in considering that
stepwise estimates are not invariant to
inconsequential linear transformation (Smith,
2018)Rather, we follow Whittingham et al.
(2006)suggestion to use a full model including all of
the effects (enter method) for the second regression
model, where it takes all multiple variables (main
variables, demographic and other determinants)
which mainly consist of either interval or categorical
properties. As a side note, this study converts all
categorical variables into dummy variables, in which
we expand each category as a new variable scored
with either one or zero.
As Pearson correlation procedure is vulnerable
from skewed and kurtosis distribution, we made
preliminary normal distribution check to avoid
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
560
inflated correlation. Each item in the questionnaire
were checked for normal distribution assumption. In
regards to normal distribution assumption, Kim
(2013) stressed that the tendency of large samples
producing inflated z in consideration to large samples
will usually produce a very small standard error for
both skewness and kurtosis. Therefore, using
skewness and kurtosis reference values for N more
than 300, the present study removed items with
kurtosis value outside the range between -7 to 7, or
skew value outside the range between -2 to 2 (Kim,
2013). After analyzing each items in the
questionnaires, this study removed four items from
EPQ idealism, which were “People should make
certain that their actions never intentionally harm
another even to a small degree”, “One should never
psychologically or physically harm another person”,
“One should not perform an action which might in
any way threaten the dignity and welfare of another
individual”, and “If an action could harm an innocent
other, then it should not be done”. Table 6 shows that
all scales from the collected data is safely within the
normal distribution bound. Thus, no transformation
for normalization is needed.
3 RESULTS
3.1 Instrument Validity
Table 7 provides the descriptive statistics for the
variables used in the analysis. All the Cronbach’s
coefficient are acceptable, ranging from a moderate
internal consistency value of 0.66 for the ‘EPQ
Idealism’ issue to a value of 0.88 for the intrinsic
personal religious orientation.
The mean score for IP was 4.22 (SD=0.53, with
maximum score of five) indicating that, overall, the
respondents considered themselves to be strongly
committed to their personal religious life. The mean
score for ES was 2.79 (SD=0.99) indicating that
overall respondents were neither strongly nor weakly
disposed towards viewing their religious practices as
an instrument for social gain.
The mean idealism score of 7.2 (SD= 1.22, with a
maximum score of 9) indicated that, in general, the
sample had a strong idealistic ethical ideology, where
they believe that their ethical behaviour will always
lead to positive consequences. The mean relativism
score was 6.29 (SD=1.46), indicating that on the
whole, the respondents believe that moral decision-
making should be situational, rather than based on
universal principles.
The ecocentric for egobiosphere values mean
score was 3.9 (SD = 0.64, maximum score of five),
indicating that as a whole, the respondents had rather
high belief in valuing the importance of the natural
environment for one’s own positive emotional effect.
The ecocentric for biosphere values mean score was
3.67 (SD = 0.66), indicating that as a whole, the
respondents had an above average belief in valuing
the importance of the natural environment. The
anthropocentric motive mean score was 3.87 (SD =
0.54) indicating that the respondents had an above
average belief in valuing the natural environment
importance for the benefit of human. Lastly, the
general environmental apathy mean score was 2.52
(SD = 0.72), indicating that the respondents had
neither strong nor weak apathy to the natural
environment.
Table 7: Descriptive statistics and measurement characteristics for variables.
Variable Scale description
Number of
items
Reliability Mean SD
ROS-Intrinsic Personal (IP) 5-point Likert-like 11 0.88 4.22 0.53
ROS-Extrinsic social (ES) 5-point Likert-like 3 0.87 2.79 0.99
EPQ Idealism 9-point Likert-like 4 0.66 7.2 1.22
EPQ Relativism 9-point Likert-like 7 0.80 6.29 1.46
Ecocentric Egobiosphere
(EEM)
5-point Likert-like 4 0.71 3.90 0.64
Ecocentric Biosphere (EBM) 5-point Likert-like 3 0.74 3.67 0.66
Anthropocentric Motives (AM) 5-point Likert-like 5 0.66 3.87 0.54
Env. Apathy 5-point Likert-like 7 0.79 2.52 0.72
*Using pearson correlation coefficient instead of Cronbach alpha, considering that the scale consists of only two items.
The Role of Religious Orientation and Ethical Ideologies in Environmental Concerns amongst Teachers and School Staff in East Java,
Indonesia
561
Table 8: Multiple regression towards egobiosphere value in ecocentric motive (EEM).
Model
EEM
b (Std. b)
Effect
Size
95% CI
Lower Upper
1 - Main Variable
A
(R=0.33; R
2
=0.11, df=9,439)
(Constant) 1.70 ** 1.330 2.077
EPQ Ideal 0.00 0.01 0.00
C
-0.029 0.039
EPQ Relative 0.04 0.11 ** 0.01
C
0.018 0.070
IP 0.43 0.35 ** 0.13
C
+ 0.351 0.499
ES 0.04 0.06 0.00
C
-0.003 0.076
2 - Main Variable + Demographic and other determinants
B
(R=0.40; R
2
=0.16, df=40, 408)
(Constant) 2.62 **
1.949 3.294
IP 0.34 0.28 ** 0.07
C
+ 0.243 0.434
How often do you visit a zoo or aquarium
1
? Once a year:
Yes (1) – No (0)
0.18 0.13 * 0.26
D
+ 0.043 0.291
How often do you visit a zoo or aquarium
1
? Once every six month:
Yes (1) – No (0)
0.22 0.10 * 0.36
D
+ 0.056 0.396
How often do you consume meat in a week
2
? I don't consume meat:
Yes (1) – No (0)
-0.23 -0.09 * 0.11
D
-0.249 0.115
What is your gender? Female
3
:
Yes (1) – No (0)
0.10 0.08 * 0.16
D
0.022 0.187
*p<.05; **p<.01;
A
regression using enter method in a stepwise manner;
B
regression using enter method, unsignificant results
omitted;
C
effect-size calculation using eta squared (F
2
);
D
effect-size calculation using Hedge’s g; +small effect size F
2
>=0.02
(or in some cases of categorical dummy variable, using Cohen’s D/Hedges’g >= 0.2); ++medium effect size F
2
>=0.15 (or in
some cases of categorical dummy variable, using cohen’s D/Hedges’g >=0.5);
1
compared to respondents who never visit
public zoo/aquarium;
2
compared to respondents who eat meat once a week;
3
compared to male respondent.
3.2 Response Rates
From 1007 total responses obtained, 78 respondents
(8%) were removed due to unengaged answers (in
other words, these were the respondents who gave the
same answer for all the questions in the
questionnaire). After the removal, there were still
some incomplete answers (listwise missing case)
from for the remaining 929 participants. Those
missing cases were imputed using a linear trend
method. In total, this research collected and analysed
929 respondents. The mean age of all respondents
(51% female (N=475) and 49% male (N=454)) is
36.38 years old (SD=10.02). The completed surveys
have a relatively balanced proportion of rural (61%)
and urban (39%) areas. Additionally, several
complementary variables were assessed, such as pet
ownership, where 48% of respondents adopted one or
more pet(s), while 52% of respondents didn’t adopt
any pet. For home ownership, 1% lived in apartment,
9% live in a rented room, 55% lived and owned a
house, while the remaining 40% still live in their
parent’s house. For the highest level of education,
74% hold a Bachelor, 14% a PhD or a Master, 8%
graduated high school, 3% hold a diploma, while for
the categories of those who either finished middle or
high school, where they either hold another degree, or
did not answer, were each less than 1%. Regarding
the frequency of zoo or aquarium visitation, 4%
visited a zoo once a month, 7% at least every six
months, 22% once a year, 42% once in every two or
more years, and lastly, 22% never visited a zoo or
aquarium, leaving the remaining 1% respondents
without answer. Regarding professions, all of the
respondents were teachers or school staff. However,
some of the respondents had a secondary profession,
as follows: 5% as an entrepreneur, 39% as an
employee in the private sector, 24% as civil servants,
5% are also scholarship students, 19% are teachers or
lecturers without a secondary profession, while the
remaining 6% are either semi-retired, social workers,
or university researchers, working in the farming or
livestock sector; others did not disclose their
professions, or did not or did not want to answer.
Finally, we also asked about the frequency of weekly
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
562
Table 9: Multiple regression towards biosphere value in ecocentric motive (EBM).
Model
EBM
b (Std. b)
Effect
Size
95% CI
Lower U
pp
er
1 - Main Variable
A
(R=0.33; R
2
=0.11, df=9,439)
(Constant) 1.23 ** 0.857 1.606
EPQ Ideal 0.03 0.06 0.00
C
-0.002 0.066
EPQ Relative 0.00 -0.01 0.00
C
-0.028 0.023
IP 0.48 0.39 ** 0.17
C
+ 0.410 0.559
ES 0.06 0.10 ** 0.01
C
0.024 0.103
2 - Main Variable + Demographic and other determinants
B
(R=0.40; R
2
=0.16, df=40, 408)
(
Constant
)
1.61**
0.907 2.304
IP 0.48 0.38 ** 0.14
C
+ 0.385 0.583
What is the highest level of schooling you have completed
1
?
Senior high: Yes (1)
No (0)
-0.26 -0.11 * 0.49
D
++ -0.509 -0.137
*p<.05; **p<.01;
A
regression using enter method in a stepwise manner;
B
regression using enter method, unsignificant results
omitted;
C
effect-size calculation using eta squared (F
2
);
D
effect-size calculation using Hedge’s g; +small effect size F
2
>=0.02
(or in some cases of categorical dummy variable, using Cohen’s D/Hedges’g >= 0.2); ++medium effect size F
2
>=0.15 (or in
some cases of categorical dummy variable, using cohen’s D/Hedges’g >=0.5);
1
compared to those respondent with
Master/PhD degree.
meat consumption whereby 6% didn’t eat meat, 28%
ate meat once in a week, 36% ate meat two to three
days in a week, 13% four to six days in a week, and
lastly, 14% ate meat every day.
3.3 Ethical Ideologies, Religious
Orientation and the Attitude
towards Natural Environment
Preservation
There are two models developed and analysed using
the multiple regression method. The first model
analyses the four main variables relation (EPQ
Idealism, relativism, intrinsic personal and extrinsic
social religious orientation) to the natural
environment protection attitude, while the second
model investigates all four main variables with all
potential demographic and other determinants taking
together as well as independently. In both of the
model, we regress all the predictors to environmental
concerns variables which are ecocentric egobiosphere
(EEM, Table 8), ecocentric biosphere (EBM, Table
9), anthropocentric motive (AM, Table 10) and
general environment apathy (GEA, Table 11).
For EEM (Table 8) the first model shows that
higher EEM score relates to a higher relativism
(b=0.04, p<0.01) and a higher IP (b=0.43, p<0.01).
However in the second model, EEM score is more
likely relate to IP (b=0.34, p<0.01), public zoo or
aquarium visitation (once a year b=0.18, p<0.01 and
once every semester b=0.22, p<0.01), gender
(b=0.10, p<0.01) and meat consumption (b=-0.23,
p<0.01).
For EBM (Table 9) the first model shows that
higher EBM score relates to a higher IP (b=0.48,
p<0.01) and a higher ES (b=0.06, p<0.01). However
in the second model, EBM score is more likely relate
to IP (b=0.48, p<0.01) and level of schooling (b=-
0.26, p<0.01).
For AM (Table 10) the first model shows that
higher EEM score relates to a higher relativism
(b=0.04, p<0.01) and a higher IP (b=0.46, p<0.01).
These relationships are replicated also in the second
model, whereby EEM score is more likely relate to a
higher relativism (b=0.04, p<0.01), a higher IP
(b=0.46, p<0.01) and older age (b=0.01, p<0.05).
However lower EEM is more likely occurred in
bachelor level of schooling compared to those of
Master/PhD (b=-0.12, p<0.05).
For GEA (Table 11), higher GEA score relates to
a lower idealism (b=-0.07, p<0.01), a higher R
(b=0.1, p<0.01), a lower IP (b=-0.25, p<0.01), and a
higher ES (b=0.17, p<0.01). However in the second
model, GEA score is more likely relate to a higher
relativism (b=0.1, p<0.01), lower IP (b=-0.26,
p<0.01), higher ES (b=0.12, p<0.01) and lower
idealism (b=-0.05, p<0.05) and level of schooling
(b=-0.26, p<0.01) along with meat consumption (four
to six day weekly (b=-0.21, p<0.05) and no meat
consumption(b=0.21, p<0.05)), household expenses
(b=0.16, p<0.05), and religious organization
affiliation (b=0.13,
p<0.05).
The Role of Religious Orientation and Ethical Ideologies in Environmental Concerns amongst Teachers and School Staff in East Java,
Indonesia
563
Table 10: Multiple regression towards anthropocentric motive (AM).
Model
AM
b (Std. b)
Effect
Size
95% CI
Lower U
pp
er
1 - Main Variable
A
(R=0.33; R
2
=0.11, df=9,439)
(
Constant
)
1.48 ** 1.183 1.783
EPQ Ideal 0.01 0.03 0.00
C
-0.014 0.040
EPQ Relative 0.04 0.12 ** 0.01
C
0.020 0.061
IP 0.46 0.45 ** 0.24
C
+ 0.404 0.524
ES 0.03 0.05 0.00
C
-0.002 0.061
2 - Main Variable + Demographic and other determinants
B
(R=0.40; R
2
=0.16, df=40, 408)
(
Constant
)
1.60**
1.053 2.147
IP 0.46 0.44 ** 0.20
C
+ 0.378 0.533
EPQ Relative 0.04 0.12 ** 0.01
C
0.015 0.063
What is
y
our a
g
e? 0.01 0.11 * 0.01
C
0.001 0.011
What is the highest level of schooling you have completed?
Bachelor: Yes (1)
No (0)
-0.12 -0.10 * 0.26
D
+ -0.243 -0.037
*p<.05; **p<.01;
A
regression using enter method in a stepwise manner;
B
regression using enter method, unsignificant results
omitted;
C
effect-size calculation using eta squared (F
2
);
D
effect-size calculation using Hedge’s g; +small effect size F
2
>=0.02
(or in some cases of categorical dummy variable, using Cohen’s D/Hedges’g >= 0.2); ++medium effect size F
2
>=0.15 (or in
some cases of categorical dummy variable, using cohen’s D/Hedges’g >=0.5);
1
compared to those respondent with
Master/PhD degree.
Table 11: Multiple regression towards general environmental apathy (GEA).
Model
GEA
b (Std. b)
Effect
Size
95% CI
Lower Upper
1 - Main Variable
A
(R=0.33; R
2
=0.11, df=9,439)
(
Constant
)
2.97 ** 2.552 3.380
EPQ Ideal -0.07 -0.11 ** 0.01
C
-0.104 -0.029
EPQ Relative 0.10 0.23 ** 0.05
C
+ 0.074 0.131
IP -0.25 -0.19 ** 0.03
C
+ -0.335 -0.171
ES 0.17 0.24 ** 0.06
C
+ 0.128 0.215
2 - Main Variable + Demographic and other determinants
B
(R=0.40; R
2
=0.16, df=40, 408)
(
Constant
)
2.91 ** 2.174 3.648
EPQ Relative 0.10 0.23 ** 0.05
C
+ 0.065 0.131
IP -0.26 -0.19 ** 0.03
C
+ -0.363 -0.155
ES 0.12 0.17 ** 0.03
C
+ 0.068 0.174
How often do you consume meat in a week
1
? Four to six days a
week: Yes
(
1
)
No
(
0
)
-0.21 -0.10 * 0.19
D
+ -0.016 0.283
What is your gross household expenses per month
2
? Refuse to
answer: Yes
(
1
)
No
(
0
)
0.16 0.10 * 0.17
D
-0.226 -0.007
EPQ Ideal -0.05 -0.09 * 0.01
C
-0.097 -0.008
Do you have any affiliation to religious organization
3
? Yes (1) –
No (0)
0.13 0.09 * 0.10
D
-0.022 0.169
How often do you consume meat in a week
1
? I don't consume
meat: Yes (1)
No (0)
0.25 0.09 * 0.20
D
+ -0.336 0.057
*p<.05; **p<.01;
A
regression using enter method in a stepwise manner;
B
regression using enter method, unsignificant results
omitted;
C
effect-size calculation using eta squared (F
2
);
D
effect-size calculation using Hedge’s g; +small effect size F
2
>=0.02
(or in some cases of categorical dummy variable, using Cohen’s D/Hedges’g >= 0.2); ++medium effect size F
2
>=0.15 (or in
some cases of categorical dummy variable, using cohen’s D/Hedges’g >=0.5);
1
compared to respondents who eat meat once
a week;
2
compared to respondent whose monthly expenses below IDR 5 million;
3
compared to those respondent who don’t
have affiliation/membership to any religious organization.
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
564
In summary, there are no evidence to support the
hypothesized relationship direction for EEM, EBM
and AM. ES is not significant with both EEM and
AM, while relativism is not significant to EBM. High
scorer of IP, however, will likely relates to a higher
EEM, EBM and AM. The higher the intrinsic
religious orientation, the more a person believes in the
importance for preserving the natural environment, in
both ecocentric and anthropocentric motives. In
addition, relativism and ES only relate to
anthropocentric motives. The higher the relativism
and extrinsic social religious orientation, the more
likely a person believes in anthropocentric values as
the motivation for preserving the natural
environment. For the second model, only in GEA that
all the main variables show consistent and stable
relationship. Higher GEA score is more likely scored
when a person scores a lower idealism, a lower
intrinsic personal religious orientation, a higher
relativism and a higher extrinsic social religious
orientation.
3.4 Extrinsic Social Religious
Orientation, Ethical Ideologies, and
Environmental Concerns
The hypothesis presented in this section is that a
higher ES correlates to lower idealism, higher
relativism, and a higher general environmental
apathy. We find only partial support for the fourth and
fifth hypothesis. The results show partial support to
the fourth hypothesis. On the one hand, to both
idealism and relativism as we found no support for
the relation of IP we also found no support in ES. It
seems that ES only positively correlates with
relativism (r[927]=0.15, p<0.01), and IP only
positively correlates with idealism (r[927]=0.21,
p<0.01). The relation of religious orientation to
environmental concerns is very similar with ethical
ideologies. The only difference is, while there is
correlation between idealism and relativism
(r[927]=0.35, p<0.01), we find no correlation
between IP and ES (Table 12).
Moreover, in Table
11, using multiple regression we confirm that higher
extrinsic social religious orientation relates to a
higher GEA in both the first and the second model.
This means that when holding all other variables
constant, one point increase in ES is more likely to
increase 0.17 point of GEA score in the first, and 0.12
point in the second model. In both models, the effect-
size of ES shows small effect-size (0.02 <= F2 <
0.15). For the confidence interval, if we were to re-fit
both models for total of 20 random trials, taking
samples of the same size from the same population,
we can be confident that for 19 out of total 20 trials
(95% of the time), an increase of one unit of ES will
be more likely to increase GEA between 0.128 to
0.215 point in the first model, while in the second
model will be more likely to increase GEA between
0.068 to 0.174 point. Therefore, except for with
idealism, the present study accepts all the expected
ES’ relations in the hypothesis.
3.5 Ethical Ideologies and Religious
Orientation
The working hypothesis presented in this section is
that higher personal religious orientation relates to a
higher idealism and a lower relativism. Table 12
provides the correlation matrix for the studied
variables. We find positive relationship between
idealism with personal religious orientation (IP)
(r[927]=0.21, p<0.01). However, there is no
significant relationship between relativism with IP
(r[927]=0.000, p>0.05), and therefore, while the
hypothesis is rejected by every relation with
relativism, it is accepted in predicting the relationship
between idealism with IP. Lastly, the correlation
between extrinsic social religious orientation and
idealism (r[927]=-0.02, p>0.05) and relativism
(r[927]=0.15, p<0.01) is already reported with a more
detail in previous section (section 3.4).
Table 12: Correlation Matrix between ROS and EPQ.
IP ES EPQ Idealism
r
CI 95%
r
CI 95%
r
CI 95%
lower upper lower upper lower upper
IP
ES 0.05 -0.02 0.11
Idealism 0.21
**
0.15 0.27 -0.02 -0.08 0.05
Relativism 0.00 -0.06 0.06 0.15
**
0.08 0.21 0.35
**
0.29 0.41
**. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
The Role of Religious Orientation and Ethical Ideologies in Environmental Concerns amongst Teachers and School Staff in East Java,
Indonesia
565
Table 13: Correlation Matrix between EASEA components.
EEM EBM AM
r
CI 95%
r
CI 95%
r
CI 95%
lowe
r
u
pp
e
r
lowe
r
u
pp
e
r
lowe
r
u
pp
e
r
Eco Egobiosphere
(
EEM
)
Eco Biosphere
(
EBM
)
0.437
**
0.384 0.488
Anthropocentric
motivation
(
AM
)
0.454
**
0.401 0.504 0.497
**
0.447 0.544
General
Environment
Apathy (GEA)
-0.113
**
-0.176 -0.049 -0.102
**
-0.165 -0.038 -0.041 -0.105 0.023
**. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
3.6 Natural Environment Preservation
Attitude (Ease)
The working hypothesis presented in this section is
general environmental apathy scale will negatively
correlated with ecocentric and anthropocentric
motives. Table 13 provides the correlation matrix for
the studied variables. We find significant correlation
in the predicted direction between general
environment apathy (GEA) with ecocentric
egobiosphere motive (EEM) (r[927]=-0.11, p<0.01),
and with ecocentric biosphere motives (EBM)
(r[927]=-0.1, p<0.01). However, there is no
significant relationship between GEA with
anthropocentric motives (AM) (r[927]=-0.04,
p<0.05).
3.7 Demographic and Other
Determinants
For all the second regression model (see Table 8 to
Table 11), aside the main variables, there are some
demographic and other determinants closely related
to environmental concerns (EEM, EBM, AM and
GEA which are gender, age, level of schooling,
weekly meat consumption, zoo visitation, monthly
expenses, and affiliation to religious organization.
While these determinants found significantly related
with environmental preservation concerns, this study
only selectively discusses those determinants with
small to medium effect-size relationship namely level
of schooling and weekly meat consumption. The
effect-size is measured using two method. For the
dummy categorical variable, we use Hedges’g in
consideration that the compared groups are different
in N and Sd. For the regressions b, we use the
cohen’s F squared (F
2
) method.
4 DISCUSSION
The present study considers that other than to ethical
ideologies, religious orientation also relates to the
attitude to the natural environment preservation. Five
general conclusions are supported by the present
study: first, two components of religious orientation
relate to ethical ideologies. Intrinsic personal
religious orientation correlates with idealism, and
extrinsic social religious orientation correlates with
relativism. This evidence leans more to the study by
Watson et al. (1998), stressing the relationship
between religious orientation and ethical ideologies,
rather than only to ethical relativism (Barnett et
al.,1996). However, in another vein, the present study
differs greatly from Watson et al. (1998), who stated
that “..intrinsicness seemed to reflect an idealistic and
antirelativistic religious identity” (p. 160). In this
study, intrinsic personal (IP) religious orientation
only relates to idealism, and extrinsic social (ES)
religious orientation only relates to relativism.
Moreover, although this study shows evidence for the
relation between idealism and relativism, there is no
correlation between IP and ES. Additionally, while it
is easy to view that, “those who expressed a strong
commitment to religious belief also believed more
strongly in universal moral principles” (Barnett et al.,
1996, p. 1169), it is important to note that both EPQ
and ROS permit a notion where those who score high
in relativism can also have a strong commitment to
religious beliefs. This is more or less proven in the
present study considering that most of the proposed
hypotheses are supported.
Second, rather than EPQ, observing results from
both first and second regression models, we find that
religious orientations (specifically IP) consistently
relate to environmental concerns. However, this study
fails to differentiate between ecocentric and
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
566
anthropocentric motives for environmental
preservation in context of their relationship with
idealism and IP. On one hand, ethical idealism is non-
significant to all ecocentric egobiosphere (EEM),
ecocentric biosphere (EBM) and anthropocentric
motives (AM), while a higher relativism is more
likely relates to a higher general environment apathy
(GEA) and AM. On the other hand, IP proves
significant indiscriminately to EEM, EBM and AM,
while ES only relates to a higher GEA. One possible
explanation for idealism and IP indiscriminate
patterns of relation towards EEM, EBM, AM may lie
in the nature of Thompson & Barton's (1994)
ecocentric and anthropocentric scale itself. As
discussed previously in the introduction, by
differentiating natural capital (the stock of
environmentally provided assets such as soil,
atmosphere, forests, water, wetlands) with cultivated
natural capital (e.g. agriculture products, pond-bred
fish, cattle herds, and plantation forests), Goodland
(1995) and Goodland & Daly (1996) explain the
dilemma of differentiating between environmental
sustainability and anthropocentric (social and
economic) sustainability. It is very easy to see that
often, there are no clear way to determine whether a
person’s environmental concerns stemmed from
ecocentric, or anthropocentric motives or both.
Moreover, even purely in ecocentric motives alone,
Amérigo et al., (2007) proves that it actually can be
divided into two factors: the egobiocentrism (self in
nature) which is more or less the element of
anthropocentric in nature and the biospherism (nature
itself). Luckily, Thompson & Barton (1994) provide
one other factor in their scale namely general
environment apathy, which we argue as one critical
aspect to differentiate whether a person has
environmental concerns or rather apathy disposition
towards their natural environment.
Third, partially accepting the working hypothesis,
both the intrinsic personal and extrinsic social
religious orientation relate consistently to the natural
environmental concerns only for the general
environmental apathy. High IP consistently relates to
a lower environmental apathy and it indiscriminately
relates to higher EEM, EBM and AM. Apparently, no
matter what the motives are (either ecocentric or
anthropocentric or both), a person with high intrinsic
personal religious orientation is more likely has a
higher concern for the natural environment
preservation. On the other hand, ES component
relates consistently to the general environmental
apathy in the hypothesised direction. A person with
high extrinsic social religious orientation is more
likely has higher environmental apathy.
Fourth, from both the first and second regression
model, this study emphasizes the strength and
reliability of religious orientation (rather than ethical
ideologies), as a more consistent factor for all the
variables designated to measure the natural
environment concerns. In addition, we find that
religious affiliation relates to general environmental
apathy.
Lastly, it is important to mention that in the result,
many of the relation between variables are small in
effect size. While effect size is critical in evaluating
whether the difference or relation is important in
terms of magnitude, by using two regression models,
this study shows consistent recurring relationships of
the main variables with environmental concerns.
Thus, despite the small effect size, these relationships
are critically important because of their consistency,
especially when all possible demographical and other
determinants compete with the main variables in the
multiple regression computation.
4.1 Ethical Ideologies and Attitudes
towards the Importance of Natural
Environment
Results for ecocentric and anthropocentric motives
shows that while idealism has no significant relation,
relativism relates to AM. However, despite the
significant relation between relativism and AM, the
effect size is very small to guarantee reliable
conclusion.
The most consistent support for the hypotheses is
shown through GEA. With small effect-size, the
results significantly show that higher relativism is
more likely relates to a higher environmental apathy.
The more the respondents view that there is no
absolute universal moral principles undergirding their
moral judgement and decision-making, they are more
likely to have higher score of environmental apathy.
In other study about animal protection and welfare,
higher relativism significantly correlated with higher
acceptability for harming animals (Bègue & Laine,
2017; McPhedran, 2009; B Su & Martens, 2017)
whereby a high score of ethical idealism is more
likely related to a lower acceptability for harming
animals (B Su & Martens, 2017). Despite animals and
ecology are not the same, environmental beliefs may
transform general ecocentric values into negative or
positive attitudes to one specific environmental
category (Bjerke & Kaltenborn, 1999). Moreover, the
similarity between findings of attitudes towards the
natural environment and animals both showing
consistent patterns relativism, suggest that it is
necessary to cross-examine such findings.
The Role of Religious Orientation and Ethical Ideologies in Environmental Concerns amongst Teachers and School Staff in East Java,
Indonesia
567
In addition, through the second model, this study
offers a new insight of the reduced strength of ethical
ideologies as one of the predictors for environmental
concerns compared to other competing factors. With
the account of demographic and other determinants,
this study shows that ethical ideologies are not as
consistent as religious orientation in predicting
environmental concerns. Rather than ethical
ideologies, this study proposes religious orientation
as a stronger and reliable factor as the predictors for
ecocentric and anthropocentric motives of
environmental concerns.
4.2 Religious Orientation and Attitudes
towards the Importance of the
Natural Environment
White (1967) marked a milestone in which research
on religious allegiance towards environmental
sustainability started. Ever since, a many studies
shows both supporting (Arbuckle & Konisky, 2015;
Barker & Bearce, 2013; Eckberg & Blocker, 1989;
Hope & Jones, 2014; Muñoz-García, 2014) and
opposing evidences (Boyd, 1999; Hayes &
Marangudakis, 2000, 2001). The present study also
finds mixed results. For environment preservation
attitude, the present study hasn’t found any
significant evidence supporting White's (1967) thesis.
Instead, high scores of personal religious orientations
(IP) relates to a more positive ecocentric (EEM and
EBM) and anthropocentric motives (AM) in valuing
the natural environment, and a lower general
environmental apathy (GEA). Rather than hindering,
religious belief and the degree to which religion is
internalized into respondents’ everyday conduct
promote respondents’ perceptions for the importance
of the natural environmental preservation. By way of
explaining this mixed result, the present study
suggests that individuals’ interpretation of religious
scripture as the result of communication framing may
be important (Feinberg & Willer, 2013; Wardekker et
al., 2009). One study points out that reframing
environmental discourse in multiple religious
teaching interpretations reduces the gap in
environmental concern between liberals and
conservatives (Feinberg & Willer, 2013). In another
study, religious framing of climate change resonates
with the electorates of both progressive and
conservative politicians and serves as a bridging
device for bipartisan climate-policy initiatives
(Wardekker et al., 2009). Hence, this study suggests
that providing information about, or controlling for,
multiple religious teaching scenarios is important to
further explaining variation between different
research results.
On the other hand, in the present paper, support
for White's (1967) thesis (that religion depresses
concern for the environment) is only found in the
relation between social religious orientation and
environmental apathy. High scorers of extrinsic social
religious orientation are more likely to have higher
general environmental apathy. The construct of ES
implies religion serves as an instrument for social
gain, exemplified by the membership of a powerful
in-group, providing protection, consolation and social
status, allowing religious participation, or use of an
ego defence (Allport & Ross, 1967; Fleck, 1981;
Genia & Shaw, 1991; Kahoe & Meadow, 1981;
Maltby, 1999). Thus, ES properties appear to more
closely resemble the embodiment of social identity
theory, rather than that of religious belief and
commitment. Therefore, the present study may
actually reveal how the social identity aspects of
religion (for example, religious group affiliation,
participation, and the like) can hinder concern for the
environment. Lastly through the second regression
model, the present study stressed the consistent
relationship between religious orientation with the
natural environment preservation motives. Even
when taking into account all other variables including
demographic and other important determinants,
religious orientation remained consistent in
predicting the concerns for the natural environment
preservation.
4.3 ROS, EPQ and Attitudes towards
the Importance of the Natural
Environment
Other than unearthing important evidence for ethical
relativism, perhaps one of the more significant
contributions from the present study is that it
examines also the main correlation of religious
orientation components (IP and ES) and ethical
ideology components (idealism and relativism).
Contrary to prediction, IP does not have a significant
relationship with relativism. This is surprising
considering that intrinsic personal religious
orientation puts religion as a deeply personal belief,
and that the sample mean indicates that most of the
respondents consider themselves to be very strongly
committed to their religious beliefs (IP Mean of 4.22
with maximum score of five). This suggests that
having a strong, deep religious belief and
commitment does not necessarily mean that
respondents consider those as their sole governing
universal moral guiding principle for their judgement
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
568
and decision-making. Furthermore, IP correlates with
idealism (Forsyth, O’Boyle, & McDaniel, 2008). This
may suggest that rather than operating as the extent to
which an individual believes in universal governing
moral principles (low relativism), intrinsic personal
religious motives, belief and commitment may relate
more to a principle with which individuals portray
and justify their actions as correct, in order to achieve
desirable outcomes (high idealism).
Second, ES relates to relativism. The more
individuals view their religious belief, participation,
and practices as the means to an end for social
motives and affiliation (for example, as group
protection, group status, or other means of social
gain), the more likely they are to have high relativism.
High relativistic individuals’ moral judgments are
adaptable, for they base their appraisals on features of
the particular situation and action they are evaluating.
People who express low relativism, in contrast, have
more cognitive beliefs in universal moral principles,
and use them to make judgements and decisions
(Feinberg & Willer, 2013, p. 815).
Moreover, it is interesting to note that an
unexpected positive correlation is found between
idealism and relativism (r[927]=0.29, p<0.01). This is
contrary to the original EPQ study which suggests
that the two scales are essentially orthogonal (Barnett
et al., 1996; Forsyth, 1980). Moreover, this
unexpected correlation was also shown in Barnett et
al. (1996) when investigating the relation between
EPQ and religiousness. Their study suggested
consistent evidence of the psychometric limitations of
ethical idealism and relativism constructs when
presented and measured on a single scale (Forsyth et
al., 2008).
Lastly, when taking into account of all the main
variables with demographic and other important
determinants, the results stress the importance of
ethical relativism, and religious orientation as the
main variables that relates to environmental concerns.
However, only intrinsic personal religious orientation
strongly relates to all measurement component for
environmental concerns (EEM, EBM, AM and GEA)
which clearly rejects White (1967) thesis. In contrast,
specifically in GEA, the result that extrinsic personal
religious orientation relates to higher environmental
apathy clearly in line with White (1967) thesis.
4.4 Demographics and Other
Determinants
Age, gender, and level of schooling are often found
to be significant demographic determinants in most
studies of religion (Arbuckle & Konisky, 2015;
Barker & Bearce, 2013; Smith & Leiserowitz, 2013)
and environment (Boyd, 1999; Hayes &
Marangudakis, 2000; Ignatow, 2006; Wolkomir,
Futreal, Woodrum, & Hoban, 1997). This study adds
public zoo visitation, meat consumption, monthly
household expenses, and religious organization
affiliation as other determinants that relate to
environmental concerns. However, this study finds
only level of schooling and weekly meat consumption
that have the ideal effect-size for a more detailed
explanation and discussion (see Table 8 to Table 11).
Results shows that compared to respondents who
consume meat once a week, respondents who
consume meat four to six times in a week have a
lower environmental apathy while respondents who
don’t consume meat tend to have a higher
environmental apathy. We propose to explain this
result through the respondentssocio-economic status
more often represented with monthly income and
expenses indicators. Unless this result originates from
being conscious of leading a healthy life, or from the
motive to preserve the natural environment,
answering no meat consumption in their daily diets
voices a very different meaning when it is in the
context of low monthly income category. On monthly
income the present study finds no significant relation
in the regression model, but, on monthly expenses, we
find relations between GEA and refuse to answer
monthly expenses group (b=0.16, p<0.05).
Respondents who refuse to answer their monthly
expenses tend to have higher general environmental
apathy compared to respondents whose monthly
expenses are below IDR five million. Thus, we
continue to examine between-group difference using
ANOVA. This study finds significant difference
between income categorical groups (F[5]=3.24,
p=0.007). Post-hoc tests using Bonferroni method
shows only one significant higher GEA in the
minimum monthly wage compared to the average
monthly income group categories (p=0.004). For
monthly expenses, this study finds significant
difference between monthly expenses categorical
groups (F[5]=2.507, p=0.029). However post-hoc
Bonferroni test fails to show any significance
difference between monthly expenses groups. One
possible cause may rest in how this study allows
participants to choose ‘refuse to answer’ option to
answer the monthly income and expenses question. It
is possible that respondents from both highest and
lowest monthly income may refuse to answer this
specific question, and thus, blurs whatever group
difference that may be found otherwise. Therefore,
this study does not yet have a sufficient explanation
other than to carefully propose that meat consumption
The Role of Religious Orientation and Ethical Ideologies in Environmental Concerns amongst Teachers and School Staff in East Java,
Indonesia
569
may warrant further investigation by examining how
it may relate to monthly income and expenses.
The present study also indicates that the level of
schooling correlates with ecocentric and
anthropocentric motives. Specific to this, result
shows that compared to respondents with a
Master/PhD degree, those respondents who finished
senior high as their last level of schooling have higher
EBM and those respondents who finished bachelor
degree as their last level of schooling have higher
AM. One probable explanation may lie in the role and
nature of those teachers who only finished senior high
compared to those teachers with bachelor degree. All
of the teacher who only completed senior high level
of schooling are situated in elementary madrasah
(religious-based elementary school) --either private
owned or formal official government school--and
function as teaching assistants. Most of them have
dual livelihood as teaching assistant and farmers
which may have higher concerns for the natural
environment. For AM, one probable explanation is
that participants with a higher, more advance degree
like Master or PhD may have more exposure and
access to environmental and animal welfare
information, compared to bachelor degree which
usually revolves more around general knowledge.
4.5 Limitation
Despite the present studys success in examining EPQ
and ROS along with influential factors for the
importance of the natural environment preservation,
it is clear that a number of other variables remain
unexplained, such as age, religious organization
affiliation, monthly household income, expenses,
public zoo/aquarium visitation, and several others.
Hence, these limitations address the need for a deeper
effort in deploying follow-up interviews to gain
insight into how those variables may have interacted
with the primary variables.
Lastly, posing animal welfare studies as one
important reference, the present study only found
partial evidence for the role of ethical ideologies in
respect to environmental concerns. However, the
remaining parts unearthed with this study is the
consistent roles of religious orientation even more
significant than the role of EPQ in animal welfare and
environmental protection studies. Moreover, previous
studies confirm that the mechanisms underlying the
relation of ethical idealism and relativism to attitudes
might vary in different countries and cultures
(Forsyth et al., 2008). The present study provides
further insight and introduce religious orientation as
contributing cultural factors that warrants further
investigation.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
We acknowledge the significance of Indonesia
Endowment Fund (Lembaga Pengelola Dana
Pendidikan Indonesia), the Faculty of Psychology
Universitas Indonesia, Rakata Adventure, Universitas
Islam Malang, Faculty of Psychology Universitas
Brawijaya, Yeka Kusumajaya, Setyo Ramadi and
KH. Ahmad Zubaidah (Gus Ida), for their enduring
support in various aspects of this research. We thank
all the respondents for their participation in this
survey. Also, the work by D. Pasaribu and P. Martens
has partly been made possible by the fellowship
‘Ethics of the Anthropocene’, Free University
Amsterdam.
FUNDING
This study has been made possible by Government of
Indonesia Ministry of Finance’s Endowment Fund for
Education Institution’s (LPDP) (Grant no
201702221010339) scholarship and funding,
https://www.lpdp.kemenkeu.go.id. The funders had
no role in study design, data collection and analysis,
decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
REFERENCES
Allport, G. W. (1966). The religious context of prejudice.
In Source: Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion
(Vol. 5). Retrieved from https://www.jstor.org/stable/
pdf/1384172.pdf?refreqid=excelsior%3A89265415bb3
88666cc896078c04361c3
Allport, G. W., & Ross, J. M. (1967). Personal religious
orientation and prejudice. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 5(4), 432–443. doi: 10.1037/h0021
212.
Amérigo, M., Aragonés, J. I., de Frutos, B., Sevillano, V.,
& Cortés, B. (2007). Underlying dimensions of
ecocentric and anthropocentric environmental beliefs.
The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10(01), 97–103.
doi: 10.1017/S1138741600006351.
Anwar, D. F. (2019). Indonesia’s democratization
underpinned by major Islamic groups and consensus
on national ideology. Retrieved February 26, 2019,
from Middle East Institute (MEI) website:
https://www.mei.edu/publications/indonesias-democra
tization-underpinned.
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
570
Arbuckle, M. B., & Konisky, D. M. (2015). The role of
religion in environmental attitudes. Social Science
Quarterly, 96(5), 1244–1263. doi: 10.1111/ssqu.12213
Bar-Tal, D., & Harel, A. S. (2002). Teachers as agents of
political influence in the Israeli high schools. Teaching
and Teacher Education, 18(1), 121–134. doi:
10.1016/S0742-051X(01)00054-3.
Barker, D. C., & Bearce, D. H. (2013). End-times theology,
the shadow of the future, and public resistance to
addressing global climate change. Political Research
Quarterly, 66(2), 267–279. doi: 10.1177/10659129
12442243.
Barnett, T., Bass, K., & Brown, G. (1996). Religiosity,
ethical ideology, and intentions to report a peer’s
wrongdoing. Journal of Business Ethics, 15(11), 1161–
1174. doi: 10.1007/BF00412815.
Bauman, W. A., Bohannon, R. R., & O’Brien, K. J. (2010).
Grounding religion: A field guide to the study of
religion and ecology. In W. A. Bauman, R. R.
Bohannon II, & K. J. O’Brien (Eds.), Grounding
Religion: A Field Guide to the Study of Religion and
Ecology. doi: 10.4324/9780203846032.
Bègue, L., & Laine, P.-J. (2017). Moral utilitarianism and
attitudes toward animals. Ethics & Behavior, 27(3),
173–178. doi: 10.1080/10508422.2016.1162720.
Bjerke, T., & Kaltenborn, B. P. (1999). The relationship of
ecocentric and anthropocentric motives to attitudes
toward large carnivores. Journal of Environmental
Psychology, 19(4), 415–421. doi: 10.1006/jevp.19
99.0135.
Blumer, H. (1958). Race Prejudice as a sense of group
position. The Pacific Sociological Review, 1(1), 3–7.
doi: 10.2307/1388607.
Boyd, H. H. (1999). Christianity and the environment in the
American public. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 38(1), 36–44.
Brundtland, G. H. (1987). Our common future: Report of
the world commission on environment and
development. In United Nations Commission (Vol. 4).
doi: 10.1080/07488008808408783.
Bush, R. (2009). Nahdlatul Ulama and the struggle for
power within Islam and politics in Indonesia. Institute
of Southeast Asian Studies.
Cornwall, M. (1989). The determinants of religious
behavior: A theoretical model and empirical test. Social
Forces, 68(2), 572–592. doi: 10.1093/sf/68.2.572.
Cornwell, B., Chi Cui, C., Mitchell, V., Schlegelmilch, B.,
Dzulkiflee, A., & Chan, J. (2005). A cross‐cultural
study of the role of religion in consumers’ ethical
positions. International Marketing Review, 22
(5), 531–
546. doi: 10.1108/02651330510624372.
Cornwell, B., Cui, C. C., Mitchell, V., Schlegelmilch, B.,
Dzulkiflee, A., & Chan, J. (1994). Religious contrasts
in consumer decision behaviour patterns: Their
dimensions and marketing implications. European
Journal of Marketing, 38, 36. doi: 10.1108/026
51330510624372.
Donahue, M. J. (1985). Intrinsic and extrinsic religiousness:
The empirical research. Journal for the Scientific Study
of Religion, 24(4), 418. doi: 10.2307/1385995.
Eckberg, D. L., & Blocker, T. J. (1989). Varieties of
religious involvement and environmental concerns:
Testing the Lynn White thesis. Journal for the Scientific
Study of Religion, 28(4), 509–517. doi:
10.2307/1386580.
Feinberg, M., & Willer, R. (2013). The moral roots of
environmental attitudes. Psychological Science, 24(1),
56–62. doi: 10.1177/0956797612449177.
Fleck, J. R. (1981). Dimensions of personal religion: A
trichotomous view. Psychology and Christianity, 66–
80.
Forsyth, D. R. (1980). A taxonomy of ethical ideologies.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(1),
175–184. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.39.1.175.
Forsyth, D. R., O’Boyle, E. H., & McDaniel, M. A. (2008).
East meets west: A meta-analytic investigation of
cultural variations in idealism and relativism. Journal
of Business Ethics, 83(4), 813–833. doi:
10.1007/s10551-008-9667-6.
Fullerton, J. T., & Hunsberger, B. (1982). A unidimensional
measure of Christian orthodoxy. Journal for the
Scientific Study of Religion, 21(4), 317. doi:
10.2307/1385521.
Gallagher, S., & Tierney, W. (2013). Religiousness/
religiosity. In M. D. Gellman & J. R. Turner (Eds.),
Encyclopedia of Behavioral Medicine (pp. 1653–1654).
New York, NY: Springer New York. doi: 10.1007/978-
1-4419-1005-9_489.
Galvin, S. L., & Herzog, H. A. (1992). Ethical ideology,
animal rights activism, and attitudes toward the
treatment of animals. Ethics & Behavior, 2(3), 141–
149. doi: 10.1207/s15327019eb0203_1.
Genia, V., & Shaw, D. G. (1991). Religion, intrinsic-
extrinsic orientation, and depression. Review of
Religious Research, 274–283.
Glock, C. Y., & Stark, R. (1965). Religion and society in
tension. Chicago. Rand McNally.
Goodland, R. (1995). The Concept of environmental
sustainability. Annual Review of Ecology and
Systematics Anniu Rev. Ecol. Syst, 26(26), 1–24. doi:
10.1146/annurev.es.26.110195.000245.
Goodland, R., & Daly, H. (1996). Environmental
sustainability: universal and non-negotiable.
Ecological Applications, 6(4), 1002–1017.
Grubb, W. N. (1995). The old problem of “new students”:
Purpose, content and pedagogy. Changing Populations,
Changing Schools. Ninety-Fourth Yearbook of the
National Society for the Study of Education, 4–29.
Haq, S. N. (2001). Islam and ecology: Toward retrieval and
reconstruction. Religion and Ecology: Can the Climate
Change, 130(4), 141–178. doi: 10.2307/20027722.
Hayes, B. C., & Marangudakis, M. (2000). Religion and
environmental issues with Anglo-American
democracies. Review of Religious Research, 42(422),
159–174. doi: 10.2307/3512527.
Hayes, B. C., & Marangudakis, M. (2001). Religion and
attitudes towards nature in Britain. British Journal of
Sociology, 52(1), 139–155. doi: 10.1080/000713100
20023073.
The Role of Religious Orientation and Ethical Ideologies in Environmental Concerns amongst Teachers and School Staff in East Java,
Indonesia
571
Herzog, H. A., & Nickell, D. (1996). Ethical ideology and
moral persuasion: personal moral philosophy, gender,
and judgments of pro- and anti-animal research
propaganda. Society & Animals, 4(1), 53–64. doi:
10.1163/156853096X00043.
Hope, A. L. B., & Jones, C. R. (2014). The impact of
religious faith on attitudes to environmental issues and
carbon capture and storage (CCS) technologies: A
mixed methods study. Technology in Society, 38, 48–
59. doi: 10.1016/j.techsoc.2014.02.003.
Hunsberger, B. (1989). A short version of the Christian
orthodoxy Scale. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 28(3), 360. doi: 10.2307/1386747.
Ignatow, G. (2006). Cultural models of nature and society.
Environment and Behavior, 38(4), 441–461. doi:
10.1177/0013916505280791.
Jackson, T. P. (1999). Naturalism, Formalism, and
supernaturalism: Moral epistemology and comparative
ethics. Journal of Religious Ethics, 27(3), 477–506. doi:
10.1111/0384-9694.00029.
Kahoe, R. D., & Meadow, M. J. (1981). A developmental
perspective on religious orientation dimensions.
Journal of Religion & Health, 20(1), 8–17. doi:
10.1007/BF01533283.
Kellstedt, L., & Smidt, C. (1991). Measuring
fundamentalism: An analysis of different operational
strategies. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,
30(3), 259. doi: 10.2307/1386972.
Kim, H.-Y. (2013). Statistical notes for clinical researchers:
assessing normal distribution (2) using skewness and
kurtosis. Restorative Dentistry & Endodontics, 38(1),
52. doi: 10.5395/rde.2013.38.1.52.
Kirkpatrick, L. A. (1999). Toward an evolutionary
psychology of religion and personality. Journal of
Personality, 67(6), 921–952. doi: 10.1111/1467-
6494.00078.
Leite, C., Fernandes, P., & Figueiredo, C. (2020). National
curriculum vs curricular contextualisation: teachers’
perspectives. Educational Studies, 46(3), 259–272. doi:
10.1080/03055698.2019.1570083.
Leong, F. T. L., & Zachar, P. (1990). An evaluation of
Allport’s religious orientation scale across one
Australian and two United States samples. Educational
and Psychological Measurement, 50(2), 359–368. doi:
10.1177/0013164490502014.
Maltby, J. (1999). The internal structure of a derived,
revised, and amended measure of the religious
orientation scale: the “age-universal” I-E scale-12.
Social Behavior and Personality, 27(4), 407–412. doi:
10.2224/sbp.1999.27.4.407.
McFague, S. (2001). New house rules: Christianity,
economics, and planetary living. Religion and Ecology:
Can the Climate Change, 130, 125–140. doi:
10.2307/20027721.
McFarland, S. G. (1989). Religious orientations and the
targets of discrimination. Journal for the Scientific
Study of Religion, 28(3), 324. doi: 10.2307/1386743.
McPhedran, S. (2009, January). A review of the evidence
for associations between empathy, violence, and animal
cruelty. Aggression and Violent Behavior, Vol. 14, pp.
1–4. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2008.07.005.
Muff, A., & Bekerman, Z. (2019). Agents of the nation-
state or transformative intellectuals? Exploring the
conflicting roles of civics teachers in Israel. Education,
Citizenship and Social Justice, 14(1), 22–39. doi:
10.1177/1746197917743752.
Muñoz-García, A. (2014). Religion and environmental
concern in Europe. Archive for the Psychology of
Religion, 36(3), 323–343. doi: 10.1163/15736121-
12341289.
Pantić, N. (2015). A model for study of teacher agency for
social justice. Teachers and Teaching: Theory and
Practice, 21(6), 759–778. doi: 10.1080/13540602.20
15.1044332.
Skipper, R., & Hyman, M. R. (1993). On measuring ethical
judgments. Journal of Business Ethics, 12(7), 535–545.
doi: 10.1007/BF00872376.
Smith, G. (2018). Step away from stepwise. Journal of Big
Data, 5(1), 32. doi: 10.1186/s40537-018-0143-6.
Smith, N., & Leiserowitz, A. (2013). American
evangelicals and global warming. Global
Environmental Change, 23(5), 1009–1017. doi:
10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2013.04.001.
Su, B, & Martens, P. (2017). Public attitudes toward
animals and the influential factors in contemporary
China. Animal Welfare, 26(2), 239–247. doi:
10.7120/09627286.26.2.239.
Su, Bingtao, & Martens, P. (2018). How ethical ideologies
relate to public attitudes toward animals: The Dutch
case. Anthrozoos, 31(2), 179–194. doi:
10.1007/s00210-018-1496-2.
Swirski, S. (2002). Politics and education in Israel:
Comparisons with the United States. Routledge.
Tajfel, Henri. (1974). Social identity and intergroup
behaviour. Information (International Social Science
Council), 13(2), 65–93. doi: 10.1177/0539018474013
00204.
Tajfel, Henri. (1981). Human groups and social categories:
Studies in social psychology. CUP Archive.
Tajfel, Henry, & Turner, J. (1979). Chapter 3 an integrative
theory of intergroup conflict. The Social Psychology of
Intergroup Relations, 33(47), 33–47. doi: 10.1016/S0
065-2601(05)37005-5.
Tan, C. (2016). Teacher agency and school-based
curriculum in China’s non-elite schools. Journal of
Educational Change, 17(3), 287–302. doi:
10.1007/s10833-016-9274-8.
Thompson, S. C. G., & Barton, M. A. (1994). Ecocentric
and anthropocentric attitudes toward the environment.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 14(2), 149–157.
doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80168-9
Tirosh-Samuelson, H. (2001). Nature in the sources of
Judaism. Daedalus, 130(4), 99–124. doi:
10.2307/20027720.
Trimble, D. E. (1997). The religious orientation scale:
Review and meta-analysis of social desirability effects.
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 57(6),
970–986. doi: 10.1177/0013164497057006007.
ICE-HUMS 2021 - International Conference on Emerging Issues in Humanity Studies and Social Sciences
572
Turmudi, E. (2006). Introduction. In Struggling for the
umma: changing leadership roles of Kiai in Jombang,
East Java (pp. 1–20). ANU Press. Retrieved from
http://www.jstor.org/stable/j.ctt2jbk2d.8.
Turner, J. C. (1975). Social comparison and social identity:
Some prospects for intergroup behaviour. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 5(1), 1–34.
Wardekker, J. A., Petersen, A. C., & van der Sluijs, J. P.
(2009). Ethics and public perception of climate change:
Exploring the Christian voices in the US public debate.
Global Environmental Change, 19(4), 512–521. doi:
10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2009.07.008.
Watson, P., Morris, R. J., Hood, R. W., Milliron, J. T., &
Stutz, N. L. (1998). Religious orientation, identity, and
the quest for meaning in ethics within an ideological
surround. International Journal for the Psychology of
Religion, 8(3), 149–164. doi: 10.1207/s15327582ijpr
0803_1.
Weaver, G. R., & Agle, B. R. (2002). Religiosity and ethical
behavior in organizations: A symbolic interactionist
perspective. Academy of Management Review, 27(1),
77–97. doi: 10.5465/amr.2002.5922390.
White, L. (1967). The historical roots of our ecological
crisis. Science (New York, N.Y.), 155(3767), 1203–
1207. doi: 10.1126/science.155.3767.1203.
Whittingham, M. J., Stephens, P. A., Bradbury, R. B., &
Freckleton, R. P. (2006). Why do we still use stepwise
modelling in ecology and behaviour? Journal of Animal
Ecology, 75(5), 1182–1189. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-
2656.2006.01141.x.
Wolkomir, M., Futreal, M., Woodrum, E., & Hoban, T.
(1997). Substantive religious belief and
environmentalism. Social Science Quarterly, 78(1),
96–108. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/
42863677.
Wuensch, K. L., Jenkins, K. W., & Poteat, G. M. (2002).
Anthrozoös a multidisciplinary journal of the
interactions of people and animals misanthropy,
idealism and attitudes towards animals. doi:
10.2752/089279302786992621.
Wuensch, K. L., & Poteat, G. M. (1998). Evaluating the
morality of animal research: Effects of ethical ideology,
gender, and purpose. Journal of Social Behaviour and
Personality, 13(1), 139–150. Retrieved from
https://repository.library.georgetown.edu/handle/1082
2/912204.
The Role of Religious Orientation and Ethical Ideologies in Environmental Concerns amongst Teachers and School Staff in East Java,
Indonesia
573