Analysis on the Influence of Family Environment on Symptoms and
Functioning of Chinese Children with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder: Based on Correlational Analysis and Independent
Samples T-Test
Zishi Li
Faculty of Brain Science, University College London, London, WCIE 6BT, U.K.
Keywords: ADHD, Family Environment, Parenting, Adaptive Behavior, Correlation-Analysis, Independent Samples T-Test.
Abstract: This research aimed to test the relationship between family environment and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder (ADHD)-related symptoms and functioning of Chinese children diagnosed with ADHD. Zhou et al.
(2005) provided evidence for significant differences in family environment ratings (E.g., cohesion,
expressiveness, conflict) between children with and without ADHD (Zhou, 2005). Building on this, the
current research investigated the extent to which family environment factors influence ADHD symptoms and
children’s adaptive behaviors. Data were extracted from the out-patient medical records of 195 children
diagnosed with ADHD at Qingdao Women and Children Hospital. The archived outcomes of the Family
Environment Scale, Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham-IV rating scale, and Adaptive Behavior Assessment System
were adopted as measurements for family environment, ADHD symptoms, and children’s adaptive behaviors
respectively. The correlation-analysis and independent samples t-test conducted by SPSS version 27. were
used to analyze the data. Pearson correlation revealed significant correlation between ADHD symptoms
(inattention and oppositional defiant) and family cohesion, organization, and conflict, which supported the
hypotheses. This laid a foundation for future investigations regarding whether and how improving the family
environment and parenting styles could help children alleviate ADHD symptoms. Nevertheless, no
correlations were found between the hyperactivity/impulsivity dimension of ADHD symptoms and any family
environment factors. Potential alternative explanations and possible modifications of methodologies in future
investigations are discussed.
1 INTRODUCTION
Being characterized by anomalous impulsivity, lack
of concentration, and motor hyperactivity, Attention-
Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is one of the
most common neurodevelopmental disorders that
onsets in childhood and usually continues into
adulthood (Price, 2012). Meta-analysis revealed that
5.29% children worldwide could be classified into
one subtype of ADHD (Polanczyk, 2007), either
predominantly inattentive (easily distractable and
disorganized), predominantly hyperactive-impulsive
(excessively fidgety and behave destructively), or
combined type (American Psychiatric Association,
2013). Using correlational analysis, this research
aimed to investigate the extent to which each family
environment subfactor relates to each dimension of
ADHD-related symptoms and adaptive behaviours,
based on the sample of Chinese children diagnosed
with ADHD.
2 LITERATURE REVIEW
Although the precise cause of ADHD remains
undefined, it was strongly approved by family and
twin studies that ADHD has a high heritability of
75% (Faraone, 2005), in which genes in the
dopaminergic pathway (E.g., DRD4, DRD5, DATI)
are involved (Stergiakouli, 2010). Nurture risk
factors mediate with nature factors in contributing to
the development of ADHD. This comprises but is not
limited to maternal stress (Price, 2012), prenatal
toxins exposure (Mill, 2008), and virus infection
(Zhou, 2015). These factors might partially explain
why ADHD is more prevalent among urban than rural
Li, Z.
Analysis on the Influence of Family Environment on Symptoms and Functioning of Chinese Children with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Based on Correlational Analysis and
Independent Samples T-Test.
DOI: 10.5220/0011731900003607
In Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on Public Management, Digital Economy and Internet Technology (ICPDI 2022), pages 145-151
ISBN: 978-989-758-620-0
Copyright
c
2023 by SCITEPRESS – Science and Technology Publications, Lda. Under CC license (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0)
145
children (Hinshaw, 1994). Family environment,
which refers to how children are raised, also received
great attention in how it profoundly affects children’s
physical, psychological, and social development, and
consequently serves as a crucial determinant of
individual’s vulnerability to ADHD. In specific, it
was demonstrated that ADHD children tend to
receive more aggressive discipline methods
(Woodward, 1998) and authoritarian parenting styles
(Lange, 2005), as compared to non-ADHD controls.
According to Pressman et al. (2006) (Pressman,
2006), inadequate parenting practices that exacerbate
ADHD symptoms could be triggered by parents
coping with multiple inner family stresses, such as
high conflict, low cohesion, and low involvement
between family members. These dimensions of
family traits are measurable through the Family
Environment Scale (FES), which examines members
interaction and culture within a family (Moos, 1984).
By applying the FES, Schroeder and Kelley (2009)
(Schroeder, 2009) found that positive parenting
(higher family cohesion, expressiveness,
organization and lower conflict) was significantly
correlated with greater behavioural regulation in
children without ADHD. Nevertheless, parenting
practices showed no significant influences on
behavioural regulation and metacognition of children
with ADHD It should be noted that the vast majority
of sample ethnicity was European-American
(67.2%), whereas a mere 3% of sample was Asian-
American. Therefore, results from Schroeder and
Kelley limit in generalizability across cultures, where
there are salient differences in child-rearing practices.
It was highlighted that while American parents
advocate children’s independent styles of thinking
and behaviours through active praising and explicit
expression of intimacy (Wilcox, 1998; Xu, 2005),
parents from non-western societies like China
emphasize on obedience to rules and adult
authorities, driven by Chinese cultural values of
Confucianism and Taoism (Chao, 1994). Zhou et al.
(2005) (Zhou, 2005) investigated the influence of
family environment on ADHD symptoms on a
Chinese sample. As a result, ADHD children scored
significantly lower on several positive dimensions of
family environment (including cohesion,
expressiveness, independence, achievement
orientation, and organization), and scored higher on
family conflict and control, as compared to non-
ADHD controls.
However, as the research from Zhou and
colleagues was conducted in the period where one-
child policy (a family pl
anning strategy to restrict
population growth) was still in place in China, all
participants recruited should be the only child in the
family. Thus, Zhou’s study could limit in the ability
of reflecting later social and economic changes (E.g.,
the multi-child policy being gradually implemented
by the Chinese government since 2011). This is due
to the significant differences in family environment
and parenting style between single-child and
multiple-child families. Child-centeredness is a
notable phenomenon under one-child policy, which
refers to caregivers devoting a large proportion of
their money, time, and effort into child development
(Chow, 1996). This causes both positive and negative
influences on the family environment. For instance,
higher expenditure on entertainments could enhance
cohesion, whereas parents’ higher expectations and
requirements on child’s education could lead to
increased familial conflict.
The current research aimed at investigating the
influence of family environment factors on ADHD
symptoms. Previous research compared between the
family environments and performances of children
with and without ADHD. As a supplementary, the
current study investigated the extent to which each
family environment factor correlates with each
dimension of ADHD symptoms and functioning of
children diagnosed with ADHD. Being carried out in
2021, this research targeted at Chinese children born
in the period that covered either the one-child policy
or the two-child policy (between 2006 and 2016).
Containing both single-child and multi-child families
in the sample could generate results that differ from
if single-child families were recruited solely. This is
because for the latter, ratings on certain factors from
FES (E.g., independence) could be consistently low,
resulting from the unique parenting of the only child
(E.g. parents devote higher cautiousness). Therefore,
for the purpose of maintaining the accuracy of
correlational patterns, samples covered both single-
child and multi-child families in the current research.
Based on what was mentioned above, 2 hypotheses
have been proposed:
Family cohesion, expressiveness, independence,
achievement orientation, and organization
would be negatively correlated with inattention,
hyperactivity/impulsivity, and oppositional
defiant
Family conflict and control would be positively
correlated with inattention,
hyperactivity/impulsivity, and oppositional
defiant.
ICPDI 2022 - International Conference on Public Management, Digital Economy and Internet Technology
146
3 METHODS
3.1 Participants
This study obtained data from the out-patient medical
record archived between November 2018 and July
2020 at Qingdao Women and Children Hospital.
Participants were 195 children diagnosed with
ADHD by 8 professional psychiatrists. 16.9% (n=33)
were girls and 83.1% (n=162) were boys. Participants
had a mean age of 8.17 (SD=1.95), ranging from 5 to
15 years old. At the time of diagnosis, 19.5% (n=38)
were in kindergarten, 76.9% (n=150) were in primary
school, and 3.6% (n=7) were in middle school. In
terms of the specific subtypes of ADHD diagnosis,
26.7% (n=52) were predominantly inattentive, 33.3%
(n=65) were predominantly hyperactive-impulsive,
and 40% (n=78) were combined type. There were
cases with comorbidities, in which 6.7% (n=13) had
Tourette Syndrome, 1.1% (n=2) had Autistic
Spectrum Disorder. 1.1% were in Obsessive
Compulsive state, while 0.5% (n=1) suffered from
Emotional Disorder. This study protected patients’
privacy by ensuring data confidentiality.
3.2 Primary Measures
The Chinese version of the Family Environment
Scale (FES-CV) was employed to quantify family
functioning (Fei, 1991). It is 90-item, true or false
self-report questionnaire filled out by the caregivers
of participants, which normally takes 20-30 minutes
to complete. The 10 subscales of family social and
environmental characteristics that FES-CV measures
are classified into three global dimensions (Moos,
1984). One dimension refers to the Social
Relationships domain, which includes the subfactors
of family cohesion (M=7.7, SD=1.9), expressiveness
(M=5.8, SD=1.7), and conflict (M=2.2, SD=1.9).
Another dimension is the Personal Growth domain,
including independence (M=5.8, SD=1.4),
achievement orientation (M=6.8, SD=1.7),
intellectual-cultural orientation (M=5.6, SD=2.1),
active-recreational orientation (M=4.9, SD=2.0),
and moral-religious emphasis (M=5.3, SD=1.4). The
third dimension is the Family System Maintenance
domain, which contains organization (M=6.7,
SD=1.8) and control (M=3.6, SD=1.8). According to
Moos and Moos (2013) (Moos, 2013), the internal
consistency reliability estimates of FES-CV had a
median of .73, which varied between .61 for
independence and .78 for cohesion, moral-religious
emphasis, and intellectual-cultural orientation. For
statistical analysis, the total score of items within
each subscale for each patient was applied.
This study collected participants’ ADHD-related
behaviours from the Chinese version of the Swanson,
Nolan, and Pelham-IV rating scale (SNAP-IV), filled
out by caregivers of patients for the purpose of
diagnosis. This 26-item rating scale measures core
ADHD symptoms defined by the Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5)
(Swanson, 1999) from three dimensions, including
Attention deficit (9 items), Hyperactivity/Impulsivity
(9 items), and Oppositional defiant (8 items). Each
item was rated on a 4-point Likert scale (0=not at all;
1=just a little; 2=quite a bit; 3=very much).
Therefore, scores could range from 0 to 78. The
SNAP-IV takes 10 minutes to complete. The
statistical analysis was based on the total score of
items from each dimension for each patient.
3.3 Secondary Measures
This study collected the level of general adaptive
functioning and skills of children with ADHD
through the parent form of the Adaptive Behaviour
Assessment System-version II (ABAS-II) (Harrison,
2000). It illustrates the General Adaptive Composite
(GAC) ability of children and three Domain
Composite Scores that encompasses nine adaptive
skill-related areas. This includes conceptual skills
(Communication, Functional academics, Self-
direction), social skills (Social, Leisure), and
practical skills (Self-care, Home/school living,
Community use, Health and safety, work). The
statistical analysis was based on the composite score
calculated from the raw score. The GAC score of
above/equal to 130 indicates outstanding
performance, and the score of below/equal to 70
indicates inferior performance. The ABAS-II was
rated by clinicians who are not involved in the
diagnosis and treatment of children, through
enquiring about children’s conditions from their
caregivers.
In addition, this study recorded patients’
intelligence scores from the Wechsler Intelligence
Scale for children (WISC) (Wechsler, 1949). The
measurement contains four primary index scores,
including verbal comprehension, fluid reasoning,
working memory, and processing speed. The total
score of above/equal to 130 indicates outstanding IQ,
while the score of below 40 indicates retardation of
IQ. Patients completed the test individually through
face-to-face interaction with a clinician who was
blind to the patient’s condition.
Analysis on the Influence of Family Environment on Symptoms and Functioning of Chinese Children with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder: Based on Correlational Analysis and Independent Samples T-Test
147
3.4 Procedure
Data extraction of this research gained permission
from the Psychology department of Qingdao Women
and Children Hospital. The demographic information
of patients was concealed when the researcher went
through the archived outpatient medical records
(paper version). The researcher collected data from
patients who completed the measurements of FES-
CV, SNAP-IV, ABAS-II, and WISC, and presented
data on the Excel spreadsheet. This research carried
out the statistical analysis using the SPSS software
version 27. Independent-samples t-test was employed
to examine gender differences on attention deficit,
hyperactivity/impulsivity, and oppositional defiant
measured by SNAP-IV. The Pearson Correlation was
used to identify the relationship between FES-CV,
SNAP-IV, and ABAS-II factors.
4 RESULTS
Building on existing finding from previous
literatures, this research investigated the degree to
which family environment factors correlates with
ADHD-related behaviors and functioning of children
diagnosed with ADHD. Independent samples t-test
was applied to examine whether there were gender
differences for each of the three dimensions of
ADHD symptoms measured by SNAP-IV. As a
result, boys scored significantly higher on
hyperactivity/impulsivity (t(193)=2.95, p=.004),
Oppositional defiant (t(193)=2.12, p=.035), and the
SNAP-IV total score (t(193)=2.15, p=.033), as
compared to girls. There were no significant gender
differences for the Attention deficit dimension,
t(193)=.47, p=.64. Potential confounding effect of IQ
was also eliminated, as it was not significantly
correlated with attention deficit (r=-.09, p=.23),
hyperactivity/impulsivity (r=.05, p=.5), or
oppositional defiant (r=-.006, p=.94).
The summary statistics of subfactors under FES-
CV and SNAP-IV are illustrated on table 1. Based on
the analyses of each FES-CV subfactor, Pearson
Correlation revealed that the SNAP-IV total score
was significantly negatively correlated with cohesion
(r=-.23, p=.001), intellectual-cultural orientation
(r=-.14, p=.04), and organization (r=-.21, p=.003);
and was significantly positively correlated with
conflict (r=.22, p=.002). Findings were consistent for
attention deficit, as it also demonstrated significant
negative correlation with cohesion (r=-.15, p=.04),
intellectual cultural orientation (r=-.17, p=.02), and
organization (r=-.21, p=.003); and significant
positive correlation with conflict (r=.22, p=.002).
However, this only partially supported the
hypotheses, as attention deficit was not correlated
with expressiveness, independence, and achievement
orientation as hypothesized. In accordance with
hypotheses, oppositional defiant was significantly
negatively correlated with cohesion (r=-.26, p<.001),
expressiveness (r=-.16, p=.03), and organization
(r=-.16, p=.02); and was significantly positively
correlated with conflict (r=.24, p<.001). However,
oppositional defiant was not correlated with
independence, achievement orientation, and control
as hypothesized. Results regarding
hyperactivity/impulsivity did not support the
hypotheses of this research, as no significant
correlation was found between
hyperactivity/impulsivity and any family environment
factors.
Table 1: Summary statistics for subfactors of FES-CV and
SNAP-IV.
Measures
Subfactors
Mean (SD)
FES-CV Cohesion
Expressiveness
Conflict
Independence
Achievement Orientation
Intellectual-cultural orientation
Active-Recreational
Orientation
Moral-Religious Emphasis
Organization
Control
6.30 (2.36)
5.03 (1.70)
4.50 (2.28)
5.20 (1.43)
5.79 (1.86)
3.90 (1.88)
4.65 (2.40)
5.22 (1.58)
5.57 (2.11)
4.56 (1.97)
SNAP-IV Attention Deficit
Hyperactivity/Impulsiveness
Oppositional Defiant
16.93(5.55)
13.11(6.44)
11.09(5.89)
This study also examined the relationship
between family environment factors and patients’
adaptive behaviour, as measured by ABAS-II.
Summary statistics are illustrated on table 2. The
General adaptation ability of patients was
significantly positively correlated with cohesion
(r=.21, p=.003), intellectual-cultural orientation
(r=.39, p<.001), active-recreational orientation
(r=.27, p<.001), and organization (r=.175, p=.015).
Similar findings were generated for practical skill, in
which it also demonstrated significant positive
correlation with cohesion (r=.21, p=.003),
intellectual-cultural orientation (r=.35, p<.001),
active-recreational orientation (r=.27, p<.001),
organization (r=.17, p=.018). Conceptual skill
showed significant positive correlation with cohesion
(r=.18, p=.012), intellectual-cultural orientation
(r=.36, p<.001), and active-recreational orientation
ICPDI 2022 - International Conference on Public Management, Digital Economy and Internet Technology
148
(r=.23, p=.001). With respect to social skill, it had
significant positive correlation with cohesion (r=.20,
p=.006), intellectual-cultural orientation (r=.34,
p<.001), active-recreational orientation (r=.25,
p<.001), organization (r=.17, p=.018), as well as
moral-religious emphasis (r=.17, p=.018).
Table 2: Summary statistics for ABAS-II.
Measures Subfactors Mean (SD)
ABAS-II General Adaptation
Conceptual Skill
Social Skill
Practical Skill
88.89 (14.31)
89.70 (14.22)
87.42 (15.26)
90.47 (14.14)
Zhou et al. (2005) (Zhou, 2005) highlighted the
role of several subfactors measured by FES-CV in
contributing to the differences in behaviours between
children with and without ADHD. This includes
family cohesion, expressiveness, independence,
achievement orientation, organization, conflict, and
control. Based on the samples of children with
ADHD, the current research supported the influences
of these factors on patients’ behaviours and
functioning, except for independence. In addition to
these factors, the current research also identified the
involvement of intellectual-cultural orientation,
active-recreational organization, and moral-
religious emphasis in affecting ADHD-related
performance. Results partially supported the
hypotheses, as family environment factors did not
exhibit any influences on the
hyperactivity/impulsivity dimension measured by
SNAP-IV. Potential alternative explanations are
discussed.
5 DISCUSSION
Although the ways in which family environment
contributes to the etiology of neurodevelopmental
disorders like ADHD received extensive research,
inconsistencies in existing findings highlighted the
necessities of further replications. With certain
modifications in the methodologies, the current study
investigated the extent to which each family
environment factor correlates with each dimension of
ADHD-related symptoms and functioning, based on
the sample of Chinese children diagnosed with
ADHD. This study demonstrated the influence of
family cohesion, organization, and conflict on both
attention deficit and oppositional defiant. The most
influential family environment factor on SNAP-IV
total score was cohesion, followed by conflict, then
by organization. Cohesion, intellectual-cultural
orientation, and active-recreational orientation also
exhibited significant influence on multiple aspects of
children’s adaptive behaviours. Intellectual-cultural
orientation was the most strongly correlated factor
with children’s general adaptation, followed by
active-recreational orientation, then by cohesion.
Nevertheless, while family environment was
found to be significantly correlated with SNAP-IV
total score, attention deficit, and oppositional defiant,
it did not exhibit any significant correlation with the
hyperactivity/impulsivity dimension of ADHD
symptoms, which was discrepant from the
hypotheses and existing findings. In addition, no
relationships were generated between attention
deficit and expressiveness, independence, and
achievement-orientation as hypothesized.
Meanwhile, independence, achievement-orientation,
and control showed no significant correlation with
oppositional defiant. Potential alternative
explanations and limitations in the methodologies are
discussed.
5.1 Alternative Explanations
One possible explanation for the lack of correlation
between hyperactivity/impulsivity and family
environment factors refers to the unbalanced gender
ratio of the research sample, as there were four times
more boys as there were girls. Findings regarding
gender differences were congruent with existing
literature (Gaub, 1997), in which
hyperactivity/impulsivity was significantly more
prevalent and was rated more frequently in boys than
in girls. This suggests that the effect of family
environment factors could be confounded by the
relatively high hyperactivity/impulsivity ratings,
which may consequently distort the outcomes of
correlational analyses. In addition, as suggested by
Rhee et al. (1999) (Rhee, 1999), genetic factors are
dominant in influencing boys’ vulnerability to
experiencing hyperactivity/impulsivity, as compared
to environmental factors. Similar patterns could not
be observed on girls.
Findings did not support significant influences of
expressiveness, independence, achievement
orientation, and control on ADHD symptoms. This
could possibly be due to the sole use of FES-CV in
assessing family environment, in which subscales for
each factor vary in reliability (with independence
being the lowest). Being relied entirely on self-
reports from parents, the FES-CV measurement
outcome could involve social desirability bias
(Fisher, 1993), as parents may refuse to display
Analysis on the Influence of Family Environment on Symptoms and Functioning of Chinese Children with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder: Based on Correlational Analysis and Independent Samples T-Test
149
possible inadequacies in their child-rearing practices
on the questionnaire. Moreover, although FES-CV
reflects the interactions between family members, it
does not control family size, socioeconomic status,
caregivers’ age, and educational background. Thus,
the comparability of parents’ ratings on family
environment factors (E.g., independence ratings for
family with 3 members vs. family with 6 members)
could be affected.
5.2 Limitations
Several inadequacies in the methodologies may
restrain the validity and reliability of findings.
Although samples recruited from the hospital covered
children attending different schools from different
regions, findings may not be generalized to children
outside the Shandong Province, where diversities of
parenting styles could be observed. The size of the
sample is limited, in comparison with the 1000
school-age children with balanced gender ratio
recruited by Khamis (2006) (Khamis, 2006). Khamis
adopted a wide range of measurements to assess the
family environment, and collected information from
multiple perspectives, including children themselves,
caregivers, and teachers. Measures were taken
includes parents’ socio-demographics (E.g.,
education, residential patterns, and household
income), children’s subjective experience of anxiety
through family interaction and degrees of satisfaction
for parental support, as well as children’s behaviour
problems rated by teachers. This enabled more
holistic and accurate analyses into how family
environment could predict ADHD performances. In
relation to the current research, measurements of
family environment and parenting styles other than
FES-CV were not available from out-patient medical
records, which implies how future studies could be
revised.
5.3 Future Investigations and
Implications
Research methodologies could be improved in further
investigations regarding the role of family
environment underlying ADHD. Larger sample with
balanced gender ratio from multiple regions in China
should be recruited. Follow-up investigations could
be conducted on children diagnosed with ADHD. A
variety of rating scales controlling parents’ socio-
demographics and patients’ personalities could be
obtained from several caregivers (E.g., grandparents)
and children themselves, to gain comprehensive
assessment of the family environment. Furthermore,
ADHD-related information could be collected from
teachers, who ideally hold better knowledge
regarding children’s attentiveness in class. Other
behavioural measurement (E.g., Child Behaviour
Checklist) (Achenbach, 1997) could be adopted in
supplement to the SNAP-IV symptom measurement,
to examine the consistency of behavioural outcomes
and enhance the validity of findings.
Through correlational analyses, the current
research provides further evidence for the influences
of family cohesion, organization, and conflict on
ADHD symptoms, in terms of attention deficit and
oppositional defiant. Findings also reveal the crucial
role of cohesion, intellectual-cultural orientation,
and active-recreational orientation underlying
children’s adaptive behaviours. In terms of
implications, the most widely adopted ADHD
interventions comprise pharmacological intervention,
behavioural intervention, and parent-training in home
settings (McGoey, 2002). After recognizing the great
influence of family environment and child-rearing
practices on the severity of ADHD symptoms, future
research could focus on whether enhancing family
cohesion and organization and reducing conflict
could lead to an alleviation of ADHD symptoms.
These modifications in the family environment could
be aided by family therapy delivered by professionals
(Everett, 2001). This would enable caregivers to
realize the critical role of themselves in influencing
children’s ADHD, as well as guide them make
improvements in parenting.
6 CONCLUSION
By conducting a correlational analysis based on
Chinese children diagnosed with ADHD, this
research investigated the extent to which family
environment influences on ADHD-related symptoms
and functioning. Possible explanations for the partial
discrepancies with hypotheses and existing findings
were discussed, which highlighted the necessity of
further investigations with modifications in
methodologies. Nevertheless, this research provided
further evidence that family cohesion is one most
critical factor underlying children’s ADHD
symptoms and related functioning. Other saliently
influential factors include organization, conflict,
intellectual-cultural orientation, and active-
recreational orientation. These findings laid a
foundation for future ADHD clinical research, in
terms of whether and how improving the environment
of families could help Chinese children alleviate
ADHD symptoms.
ICPDI 2022 - International Conference on Public Management, Digital Economy and Internet Technology
150
REFERENCES
Achenbach, T.M., & Edelbrock, C. Child behavior
checklist. Burlington (Vt), 1997(7): 371-392.
American Psychiatric Association, A. P., & American
Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. 2013(10).
Washington, DC: American psychiatric association.
Chao, R.K. Beyond parental control and authoritarian
parenting style: Understanding Chinese parenting
through the cultural notion of training. Child
development, 1994, 65(4): 1111-1119.
Chow, E.N.L., & Zhao, S.M. The onechild policy and
parentchild relationships: a comparison of onechild
with multiplechild families in China. International
Journal of Sociology and Social Policy.1996.
Everett, C.A., & Everett, S.V. Family therapy for ADHD:
Treating children, adolescents, and adults. Guilford
Press. 2001.
Faraone S.V., Perlis R.H., Doyle A.E., Smoller J.W.,
Goralnick J.J., Holmgren M.A,. Sklar P. Molecular
genetics of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol
Psychiatry, 2005(57): 1313–13
Fei, L., Shen, Q., Zheng, Y., Zhao, J., Jiang, S., Wang, L.,
& Wang, X. Preliminary evaluation of "Family
Intimacy and Adaptability Scale" and "Family
Environment Scale"-Normal Family and Spirit A
controlled study of family members with schizophrenia.
Chinese Mental Health Journal, 1991(5).
Fisher, R.J. Social desirability bias and the validity of
indirect questioning. Journal of consumer research,
1993, 20(2): 303-315.
Gaub, M., & Carlson, C.L. Gender differences in ADHD: A
meta-analysis and critical review. Journal of the
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
1997, 36(8): 1036-1045.
Harrison, P.L., & Oakland, T. Adaptive behavior
assessment system. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
Corporation. 2000.
Hinshaw, S.P. Attention deficits and hyperactivity in
children. 1994(29). Sage.
Khamis, V. Family environment and parenting as predictors
of attention-deficit and hyperactivity among Palestinian
children. Journal of Social Service Research, 2006,
32(4): 99-116.
Lange, G., Sheerin, D., Carr, A., Dooley, B., Barton, V.,
Marshall, D., & Doyle, M. Family factors associated
with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and
emotional disorders in children. Journal of Family
Therapy, 2005, 27(1): 76-96.
Mill, J., & Petronis, A. Preand perinatal environmental
risks for attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD): The potential role of epigenetic processes in
mediating susceptibility. Journal of child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 2008, 49(10): 1020-1030.
McGoey, K.E., Eckert, T.L., & Dupaul, G.J. Early
intervention for preschool-age children with ADHD: A
literature review. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral
Disorders, 2002, 10(1), 14-28.
Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. Family Environment Scale
(FES) (3rd ed.). CA: Consulting Psychologists Press
Inc. 1984.
Moos, R.H., & Moos, B.S. Family environment scale.
Handbook of measurements for marriage and family
therapy, 2013(82).
Polanczyk, G., De Lima, M.S., Horta, B.L., Biederman, J.,
& Rohde, L.A. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a
systematic review and metaregression analysis.
American journal of psychiatry, 2007, 164(6), 942-948.
Pressman, L.J., Loo, S.K., Carpenter, E.M., Asarnow, J.R.,
Lynn, D., McCracken, J.T., & Smalley, S.L.
Relationship of family environment and parental
psychiatric diagnosis to impairment in ADHD. Journal
of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 2006, 45(3): 346-354.
Price, M., & Raffelsbauer, D. ADHD: A true
neurodevelopmental disorder?. Medical Writing, 2012,
21(2): 114-122.
Rhee, S.H., Waldman, I.D., Hay, D.A., & Levy, F. Sex
differences in genetic and environmental influences on
DSM–III–R attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Journal of abnormal psychology, 1999, 108(1), 24.
Schroeder, V.M., & Kelley, M.L. Associations between
family environment, parenting practices, and executive
functioning of children with and without ADHD. Journal
of child and family studies, 2009, 18(2): 227-235.
Stergiakouli, E., & Thapar, A. Fitting the pieces together:
current research on the genetic basis of attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment, 2010(6): 551.
Swanson, J., Lerner, M., March, J., & Gresham, F. M.
Assessment and intervention for attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the schools: Lessons
from the MTA study. Pediatric Clinics of North
America,1999, 46(5): 993-1009.
Wechsler, D., & Kodama, H. Wechsler intelligence scale for
children. New York: Psychological corporation. 1949.
Wilcox, W.B. Conservative Protestant childrearing:
Authoritarian or authoritative?. American Sociological
Review, 1998, 796-809.
Woodward, L., Taylor, E., & Dowdney, L. The parenting
and family functioning of children with hyperactivity.
The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and
Allied Disciplines, 1998, 39(2), 161-169.
Xu, Y., Farver, J. A., Zhang, Z., Zeng, Q., Yu, L., & Cai, B.
Mainland Chinese parenting styles and parent-child
interaction. International Journal of Behavioral
Development, 2005, 29(6): 524-531.
Zhou, W., Luo, X., Wei, Z., & Zhu, Y. Personality and
behavior characteristics of children with attention
deficit disorder and hyperactivity and their family
environment. China Clinical Rehabilitation, 2005,
9(44): 72-74.
Zhou, R., Xia, Q., Shen, H., Yang, X., Zhang, Y., & Xu, J.
Diagnosis of children’s attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD) and its association with
cytomegalovirus infection with ADHD: a historical
review. International journal of clinical and
experimental medicine, 2015, 8(8): 13969.
Analysis on the Influence of Family Environment on Symptoms and Functioning of Chinese Children with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder: Based on Correlational Analysis and Independent Samples T-Test
151