algorithm delivers logical rules as the criteria
separating alarm omission and detection from values
on the four centered electrodes Cz, Pz, Oz and Fz.
Starting from a normalized form of these rules which
is easily explainable as a Boolean expression, we can
generate the appropriate code in a static context or in
dynamic context. In a static context, once EEG values
are available, one can predict attention failure
regardless to the software involved as the
implementation context (Python, Java, Matlab, …); in
a dynamic context, this is more interesting: since one
can define an active role for electrodes taken as agents
with a dedicated level of knowledge. That way, one
can reengineer completely these rules according to
electrodes as both actuators and sensors. That way,
one can improve the experimentation domain by of
putting the classification work in the loop thanks to a
multi-agent model. Active electrodes become virtual
agents (sensors and actuators) connected together
thanks to logical connectors as firing rules. including
other actuators (red light alarm, sounds, …). Domain-
specific scenarios and doctrines can be defined.
thanks to explainable classification. From that
situation awareness, one can expect connect more
powerful automatic decision mechanisms. In effect,
abnormal behavior detection is the first step of the
sense-making process relayed by decision-making.
For instance, the purpose is to trigger a sequence of
actions to be engaged, whether these actions are
automatic or not. As a use-case, one can mention the
situation in a cockpit characterized by a loss of
attention of the pilot and his/her inability to continue
his/her current mission. That is, the operator did not
consciously detect the alarm although his brain
processed the signal. It is therefore necessary to
inform the operator that he has omitted the alarm (by
feedback) and to adapt the work environment with the
explainable AI to help him in his task so that he comes
back in the loop.
REFERENCES
Ackerman, P. L., & Kanfer, R. (2009). Test length and
cognitive fatigue: An empirical examination of effects
on performance and test-taker reactions. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15(2), 163-181.
Barwick, F., Arnett, P., & Slobounov, S. (2012). EEG
correlates of fatigue during administration of a
neuropsychological test battery. Clinical
Neurophysiology, 123(2), 278-284.
Boksem, M. A. S., Meijman, T. F., & Lorist, M. M. (2005).
Effects of mental fatigue on attention: An ERP study.
Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 107-116.
Boksem, M. A. S., & Tops, M. (2008). Mental fatigue:
Costs and benefits. Brain Research Reviews, 59(1),
125-139.
Borghini, G., Vecchiato, G., Toppi, J., Astolfi, L.,
Maglione, A., Isabella, R., Caltagirone, C., Kong, W.,
Wei, D., Zhou, Z., Polidori, L., Vitiello, S., & Babiloni,
F. (2012). Assessment of mental fatigue during car
driving by using high resolution EEG activity and
neurophysiologic indices. 2012 Annual International
Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and
Biology Society, 6442-6445.
Dehais, F., Duprès, A., Blum, S., Drougard, N., Scannella,
S., Roy, R., & Lotte, F. (2019). Monitoring Pilot’s
Mental Workload Using ERPs and Spectral Power with
a Six-Dry-Electrode EEG System in Real Flight
Conditions. Sensors, 19(6), 1324.
Dehais, F., Dupres, A., Di Flumeri, G., Verdiere, K.,
Borghini, G., Babiloni, F., & Roy, R. (2018).
Monitoring Pilot’s Cognitive Fatigue with Engagement
Features in Simulated and Actual Flight Conditions
Using an Hybrid fNIRS-EEG Passive BCI. 2018 IEEE
International Conference on Systems, Man, and
Cybernetics (SMC), 544-549.
Dönmez, K., & Uslu, S. (2018). The Relationship Between
Flight Operations and Organizations in Aircraft
Accidents; The Application of the Human Factor
Analysis and Classification System. Anadolu
University Journal of Science and Technology A -
Applied Sciences and Engineering, 1-1.
Faber, L. G., Maurits, N. M., & Lorist, M. M. (2012).
Mental Fatigue Affects Visual Selective Attention.
PLoS ONE, 7(10), e48073.
Inzlicht, M., Schmeichel, B. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2014).
Why self-control seems (but may not be) limited.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(3), 127-133.
Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J.
(2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort
and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
36(6), 661-679.
Lee, K. A., Hicks, G., & Nino-Murcia, G. (1991). Validity
and reliability of a scale to assess fatigue. Psychiatry
Research, 36(3), 291-298.
Lorist, M. M., Boksem, M. A. S., & Ridderinkhof, K. R.
(2005). Impaired cognitive control and reduced
cingulate activity during mental fatigue. Cognitive
Brain Research, 24(2), 199-205.
Marcus, J. H., & Rosekind, M. R. (2017). Fatigue in
transportation : NTSB investigations and safety
recommendations. Injury Prevention, 23(4), 232-238.
Möckel, T., Beste, C., & Wascher, E. (2015). The Effects
of Time on Task in Response Selection—An ERP
Study of Mental Fatigue. Scientific Reports, 5(1),
10113.
Müller, T., & Apps, M. A. J. (2019). Motivational fatigue :
A neurocognitive framework for the impact of effortful
exertion on subsequent motivation. Neuropsychologia,
123, 141-151.
Sabeti, M., Boostani, R., & Rastgar, K. (2017). How mental
fatigue affects the neural sources of P300 component?
Journal of Integrative Neuroscience, 17, 1-19.